Foraging in honeybees is energetically demanding. Here, we examined whether stressors, which generally increase metabolic demands, can impair foraging performance. A controlled non-pathogenic stressor (immune challenge) resulted in a change in the foraging preferences of bees. It reduced pollen foraging and increased the duration of trips in pollen foragers. Stress also reduced the amount of octopamine in the brain of pollen foragers (a biogenic amine involved in the regulation of foraging and flight behaviour in insects). According to the literature, flight metabolic rate is higher during pollen foraging than during nectar foraging, and nectar gives a higher energetic return relative to the foraging effort when compared with pollen. We thus propose that stress might be particularly detrimental to the performance of pollen foragers, and stressed bees prefer the energy-rich resource of nectar. In conclusion, stress, even at low levels, could have consequences for bee foraging behaviour and thereby the nutritional balance of the colony.

For honeybees, which are central-place foragers relying on pollen and nectar from flowers, foraging behaviour places demands on both cognitive capacity (Klein et al., 2017) and metabolic capacity: indeed, insect flight is known to be among the most intense and energy-demanding physiological processes in the animal kingdom (Dudley, 2000). The metabolic rates of flying insects, mainly fuelled by carbohydrates, can be up to 170 times higher than those of resting individuals (Bartholomew and Casey, 1978). As a consequence, it is expected that environmental stressors (e.g. parasites and temperature changes), which often impose increased metabolic demands (Bordier et al., 2017a; Johnson and White, 2009), could compromise foraging performance. Deciphering how stress impacts honeybee foraging performance might therefore help us better understand the mechanisms underlying colony decline and failure, which continues to be an issue of widespread concern (Goulson et al., 2015; Potts et al., 2010).

Stressors may directly limit bees' energetic reserves and thus reduce foraging performance. Indeed, there are several reports of a reduction of global flight activity in parasitized bees due to energy depletion (Kralj and Fuchs, 2010; Alaux et al., 2014; Naug, 2014; Wolf et al., 2014). Stressors may also affect forager decision-making processes as a consequence of the energetic challenges of the stressor, in which case bees may show a preference for carbohydrate-rich resources to supply their own energy needs. The finding that the gene coding for the pheromone biosynthesis-activating neuropeptide, a neuropeptide known to be present at higher levels in nectar foragers than in pollen foragers (Brockmann et al., 2009), is over-expressed in parasitized bees (McDonnell et al., 2013) provides some indirect support for this hypothesis. Stress can decrease sucrose responsiveness (Pankiw and Page, 2003), which is lower in nectar foragers than in pollen foragers (Pankiw and Page, 2000), suggesting that stress might cause a change in foraging preference. In addition, it has been shown that parasitized bees are less likely to forage for pollen (Lach et al., 2015). Together, these findings suggest that stressed bees may favour nectar over pollen foraging. This could have consequences for the nutritional balance and development of the colony, as the majority of larva protein intake indirectly comes from pollen supply (Brodschneider and Crailsheim, 2010; Pernal and Currie, 2000). Moreover, pollen nutrition promotes immunocompetence and parasitism tolerance of adult bees (Alaux et al., 2010; Di Pasquale et al., 2013).

To test the hypothesis that stress can induce a change in foraging performance, without any potential effects of parasite manipulation of host metabolism (Adamo, 2012; Biron and Loxdale, 2013), we exposed bees to a non-pathogenic immune challenge. Immune responses are energetically costly, and even simple responses, like encapsulation, can raise metabolic rate by up to 28% in insects (Ardia et al., 2012; Freitak et al., 2003). We then tracked their foraging behaviour throughout their life with a radio-frequency identification device (RFID), and a camera at the colony entrance to identify whether they carried pollen loads. Finally, we assessed the influence of stress on brain biogenic amine levels, which are known to be involved in the regulation of bee behaviour (Schulz and Robinson, 2001; Schulz et al., 2002).

Experiments were performed from January to April 2016 with honeybees (Apis mellifera Linnaeus 1758) obtained from the research apiary at Macquarie University (Sydney, NSW, Australia). We tested the influence of stress on foraging behaviour (experiment 1) and brain biogenic amine signalling (experiment 2). Frames containing late-stage pupae were collected from three donor colonies and placed into an incubator overnight at 34°C. Newly emerged bees were marked on the thorax with either a RFID tag for experiment 1 or a paint mark for the experiment 2, and released into host colonies. They were then re-captured when 7 days old and placed in plastic cages with ad libitum sugar solution (50% w/v). Half of the bees were given an immune challenge, which consisted of piercing the cuticle between the third and the fourth tergites of the abdomen using a 0.15 mm needle. If a haemolymph drop was released after the pin prick, the bee was discarded. Previous studies have shown that the bee's immune system is activated by this wounding alone, without pathogen infection (Alaux et al., 2014; Evans et al., 2006; Siede et al., 2012). Control bees did not receive any pin prick. Handled bees (control and immune challenged) were given an additional paint mark on the abdomen to identify them by their treatment group before they were released back into their colony. This procedure was repeated three times with different bees.

Experiment 1: impact of immune challenge on foraging performance

Following the stress treatment at 7 days, 380 control and 370 stressed bees in total (n=3 trials) were released into a small nucleus hive equipped with a modified entrance. Bees had to use a specific path to exit the hive and another one to enter the hive. Each path was made of transparent 1 cm diameter plastic tubing (Bunnings, Gordon, NSW, Australia). To avoid bees using the wrong path, a plastic gate with plastic bristles, which bees could use in only one direction, was placed at the end of each path. The traffic of bees was also regulated using infrared-activated gates placed at the beginning of each path (Arduino Technology, Arduino, Adafruit and Little Bird Electronics, Hornsby, NSW, Australia). Each time a bee broke the infrared beam, the linked gates were closed behind the bee for 10 s, which was the time needed for bees to cross the path and RFID system. Each path was equipped with a RFID reader (Invengo, Guangzhou, China; Perry et al., 2015; Søvik et al., 2015) to monitor each of the entrance and exit channels. Each RFID tag (diameter 4 mm, mass 1 mg) had a unique digital identifier read by the antennae at the entrance and exit. The entrance path was also equipped with a digital video camera (Logitech, Lausanne, Switzerland) and a white LED light enclosed in a plastic box. Motion detection video recording software (ZoneTriger, Omega Unfold Inc., Montreal, QC, Canada) was used to visually identify whether bees carried pollen or not.

Experiments continued until the last recording of the last bee, i.e. 55 days. RFID data, i.e. bee ID, direction (entry into or exit from the hive) and time (day, hours, minutes and seconds), were recorded in .csv files. From these data, we were able to reconstruct trips outside the hive for each bee. RFID readings were time matched with readings from the camera, and videos taken from 10 s before RFID detection were inspected to identify the resource for the returning bees (pollen or not pollen). Only data for bees with an RFID tag and paint marks on their abdomen were analysed. Trips shorter than 30 s were not considered as foraging flights and were excluded from the study. As in Perry et al. (2015), trips longer than 8 h were also removed.

Of the 380 control and 370 immune-challenged bees, a completed foraging flight was recorded at least once from 96 and 74 bees, respectively. This loss of bees could be due to the loss of tag prior to leaving the hive, ejection from the colony by nestmates or death of the bee during its first flight. In total, 979 flights identified as pollen (n=154) or non-pollen (which can be nectar, water or an empty crop; n=825) foraging flights were recorded. The number of foraging flights appeared to be relatively low for a total of 170 bees, but was probably explained by the fact that the experimental device contained only one entry and one exit path (one bee at a time could use the path), and that many bees completed a very low number of flights (median, first and third quartiles: 4, 2, 8 foraging trips per bee, respectively). A maximum of 83 completed foraging trips per bee was recorded and 20 bees completed more than 20 trips.

Experiment 2: impact of immune challenge on brain biogenic amine levels

After the stress treatment on day 7, 637 control and 695 immune-challenged bees in total were introduced into a normal Langstroth colony (n=3 trials). Bees returning to the colony when they were between 24 and 28 days old were sampled and immediately flash-frozen in liquid nitrogen. Whether they carried pollen or not was also noted. Frozen heads were freeze-dried for 60 min at a pressure below 300 mTorr (∼40 Pa; VirTis BenchtopTM) and −35°C and then stored at −80°C until brain dissection and biogenic amine analysis. Brain dissections (including optic lobes, antennal lobes, the central brain and gnathal ganglion) were performed on dry ice.

Brain biogenic amine (octopamine, OA; dopamine, DA; tyramine, TYR; and serotonin, 5-HT) levels were measured using high-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC) following the protocol described by Søvik et al. (2013) and also used later (Scheiner et al., 2014; Søvik et al., 2015). Briefly, the HPLC system was composed of a pump and an autosampler (Agilent 1200 Series, Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA), coupled to an electrochemical detector (ESA Coulochem III, Chelmsford, MA, USA) connected to an analytical cell (ESA 3011A). A 100 mm Hypersil BDS octadecylsilane column was used to separate samples (ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). Signals were integrated using ChemStation software (Agilent Technologies) with reference to a standard curve obtained from perchloric acid solutions containing 10 pg µl−1 of dihydroxybenzylamine and varying amounts of OA, DA, TYR and 5-HT (Sigma-Aldrich).

In total, we obtained information on brain levels of biogenic amines for 94 control bees (32 with pollen and 62 without pollen) and 50 immune-challenged bees (12 with pollen and 38 without pollen). TYR was detected in only 14% of brains, and thus was not analysed.

Statistical analysis

All statistics were performed using the statistical software R version 3.2.1 (http://www.R-project.org/). For experiment 1, the last day any individual bee was detected using RFID was assumed to mark the date of bee death. We then compared the probability of survival between stressed and control bees using the Kaplan–Meier test (‘surfit’ function of the survival package in R) (Therneau and Lumley, 2014).

Aspects of the foraging performance of bees were analysed using mixed models. The choice of best-fit model was based on the smaller sample size-corrected Akaike's information criterion (AICc) (Burnham and Anderson, 2004). Variation in total number of completed foraging flights per bee, the collected resource (pollen or not pollen) and foraging trip duration were each analysed using different mixed models and fitted with a Poisson, binomial and Gaussian distribution, respectively (based on the distributions of our experimental data). To analyse the number of trips and the collected resource, the treatment (immune challenged or control) and trial were set as fixed and random explanatory variables, respectively. To analyse foraging trip duration, collected resource and honeybee identification were added as fixed and random explanatory variables, respectively.

The normality and the homoscedasticity of brain biogenic amine levels were such that parametric analyses were appropriate for these data. Biogenic amine amounts were analysed using a repeated measures ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc comparison. Treatment and the resource collected (pollen or not pollen) were analysed as fixed factors, while trial was analysed as a random factor.

Experiment 1: impact of immune challenge on survival and foraging performance

Survival probability did not differ between the control and immune-challenged groups (Kaplan–Meier test, P=0.42; Fig. 1A).

Fig. 1.

Survival probability and foragingtrip characteristics according to treatment. (A) Survival over 49 days for control bees and immune-challenged bees. Day 0 was the day of stress exposure. Bees from the two treatment groups did not differ in survival probability (Kaplan–Meier test, P=0.42). (B) Percentage of pollen and non-pollen foraging trips. (C) Foraging trip duration. For B and C, the mean and 95% confidence intervals predicted by the model (Table 1) are shown according to the collected resource and the treatment: control (n=100 pollen and 401 non-pollen foraging trips) and immune challenge (n=54 pollen and 424 non-pollen foraging trips). Immune-challenged bees performed fewer but longer pollen foraging trips than control bees.

Fig. 1.

Survival probability and foragingtrip characteristics according to treatment. (A) Survival over 49 days for control bees and immune-challenged bees. Day 0 was the day of stress exposure. Bees from the two treatment groups did not differ in survival probability (Kaplan–Meier test, P=0.42). (B) Percentage of pollen and non-pollen foraging trips. (C) Foraging trip duration. For B and C, the mean and 95% confidence intervals predicted by the model (Table 1) are shown according to the collected resource and the treatment: control (n=100 pollen and 401 non-pollen foraging trips) and immune challenge (n=54 pollen and 424 non-pollen foraging trips). Immune-challenged bees performed fewer but longer pollen foraging trips than control bees.

The best-fit model explaining the variability in the number of trips per bee (lowest AICc) included a significant effect of treatment (Table 1). Immune-challenged bees completed slightly more flights than control bees [mean predicted values with 95% confidence interval: 6.46 (6.12–6.80) versus 5.22 (4.95–5.49), respectively].

Table 1.

Summary of best-fit mixed models to analyse the impact of immune challenge on foraging behaviour

Summary of best-fit mixed models to analyse the impact of immune challenge on foraging behaviour
Summary of best-fit mixed models to analyse the impact of immune challenge on foraging behaviour

A significant switch in foraging preference was detected, with immune-challenged bees performing 1.9 times fewer pollen foraging trips [9.14% (8.32%–9.96%)] than control bees [17.56% (16.20%–18.91%); Fig. 1B and Table 1].

Considering foraging trip duration, the best-fit model included a significant interaction between treatment (immune challenged or control) and the collected resource (pollen or not pollen) (Table 1). Pollen foraging trips were longer than non-pollen foraging trips (Fig. 1C), but trip duration for each collected resource also varied with treatment. Immune-challenged bees performed slightly shorter non-pollen foraging trips than control bees (Fig. 1C), but when foraging for pollen, immune-challenged bees performed 30% longer trips than control bees (Table 1).

Experiment 2: impact of immune challenge on brain biogenic amine levels

Brain DA and 5-HT levels did not differ significantly between treatment groups (ANOVA: P=0.67 and P=0.14, respectively) or the collected resource (ANOVA: P=0.75 and P=0.27, respectively; Fig. 2A,B). However, we found a significant treatment×resource interaction for brain OA levels (ANOVA: P=0.02; Fig. 2C). No difference in brain OA levels was found in non-pollen foraging bees (Tukey's post hoc tests: P=1); however, when sampled on return to the hive carrying pollen, immune-challenged bees had significantly less OA in the brain than control bees (∼27% less, Tukey's post hoc tests: P=0.032; Fig. 2C).

Fig. 2.

Brain biogenic amine levels in response to immune challenge in different forager groups. Dopamine (DA, A), serotonin (5-HT, B) and octopamine (OA, C) levels are shown for control (n=32 pollen and 62 non-pollen foragers) and immune-challenged bees (n=12 pollen and 38 non-pollen foragers), according to the collected resource. Boxes show the first and third interquartile range with the line denoting the median. Whiskers encompass 90% of the individuals, beyond which each outlier is represented by a circle. Plots that do not share a common letter are statistically different (P<0.05, ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc comparisons).

Fig. 2.

Brain biogenic amine levels in response to immune challenge in different forager groups. Dopamine (DA, A), serotonin (5-HT, B) and octopamine (OA, C) levels are shown for control (n=32 pollen and 62 non-pollen foragers) and immune-challenged bees (n=12 pollen and 38 non-pollen foragers), according to the collected resource. Boxes show the first and third interquartile range with the line denoting the median. Whiskers encompass 90% of the individuals, beyond which each outlier is represented by a circle. Plots that do not share a common letter are statistically different (P<0.05, ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc comparisons).

Experimental findings

In this study, we have provided experimental evidence for a stress-induced decrease in pollen-foraging performance in honeybees. The non-pathogenic immune challenge stress applied did not affect bee survival, as has been found previously (Alaux et al., 2014), but did induce a shift in resource collection. An increase in non-pollen foragers (water foragers, nectar foragers and/or bees with empty crops) was observed at the expense of pollen foragers. As more than 90% of non-pollen foragers are nectar foragers and bees with empty crops (Bordier et al., 2017b) and these bees have lower sucrose responsiveness than pollen foragers (Pankiw and Page, 2003), we could reasonably assume that stress decreased bee sucrose responsiveness. Stressed bees may prefer to forage for resources that are rich in carbohydrates to overcome the energetic cost of the stress, as has been observed with parasitism of honeybees (Lach et al., 2015). Indeed, compared with pollen, nectar gives a higher energetic return relative to the foraging effort (8:1 gain with pollen versus 10:1 gain with nectar; Winston, 1987). Similarly, bumblebees exposed to pesticides were found to exhibit lower pollen foraging performance (Feltham et al., 2014; Gill and Raine, 2014).

Such changes in foraging decision making could cause a nutritional imbalance with a pollen deficit at the colony level, and thereby affect colony development. Indeed, pollen shortage may have detrimental effects on brood care, resulting in undernourished larvae (Blaschon et al., 1999) and emerging adults with behavioural deficiencies (Scofield and Mattila, 2015). Moreover, pollen nutrition during the adult stage is essential for stress tolerance (DeGrandi-Hoffman et al., 2010; Di Pasquale et al., 2013; Wahl and Ulm, 1983). Finally, under extreme pollen shortage, nurse bees may reduce the number of larvae that need to be fed, and cannibalize eggs and young larvae (Schmickl and Crailsheim, 2001).

Pollen foraging trips were also 30% longer for immune-challenged bees, suggesting a significant effect of the stressor on foraging capacity. It has been found that the thorax temperature differs between different classes of foragers, in the order pollen>nectar>water foragers (Feuerbacher et al., 2003). These differences were linked to flight metabolic rate, with pollen foragers exhibiting a 10% higher hovering metabolic rate than nectar foragers, regardless of their loads (Feuerbacher et al., 2003). The authors suggested that pollen foragers require more power output to generate the same vertical lift as nectar foragers. We therefore propose that immune-challenged bees spend more time on pollen-collecting trips because it is the most energetically demanding resource to collect (Feuerbacher et al., 2003) and the stressor probably decreases the energy budget of the bees. The increase in foraging trip duration may simply reflect more time resting rather than any other changes in flight characteristics (e.g. distance, speed, etc.) (Wolf et al., 2014). It is also possible that a lower energy budget induced by the stressor caused cognitive impairment in pollen foragers and thus affected their navigation capacities (Jaumann et al., 2013), lengthening their trip times.

Finally, we found that brain OA level was depressed in immune-challenged pollen foragers. OA is known to increase sucrose responsiveness in bees (Scheiner et al., 2002) and stimulate flight activity (Fussnecker et al., 2006), and therefore the drop in OA level is in accordance with the behavioural changes observed in pollen foragers after stress exposure. A previous study reported a rapid decrease in OA and DA but not 5-HT levels in response to stress exposure (chilling anaesthesia and vertical spin; Chen et al., 2008). We did not find variation in DA levels after our stress exposure. However, to conclude on the nature of the causal role of biogenic amines in honeybee stress responses, functional studies involving manipulation of OA, DA and 5-HT signalling would be required.

Our study suggests that the highly energy-demanding foraging activity of pollen foragers makes them susceptible to stress, even at low levels, which could potentially affect the colony nutrient balance (pollen versus nectar). Therefore, future studies on whether stress narrows the colony foraging flexibility in response to environmental changes might help us to better understand colony decline.

We thank F. Kamhi and B. V. Entler for their help with laboratory work, A. Cabirol for her help with fieldwork, and H. Dechatre and A. Faure for their help in data analyses.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: C.B., A.B.B., C.A.; Methodology: C.B., S.K.; Formal analysis: C.B., S.K.; Resources: A.B.B.; Writing - original draft: C.B., C.A.; Writing - review & editing: C.B., S.K., Y.L.C., A.B.B., C.A.; Supervision: A.B., C.A.

Funding

C.B. was supported by an ANR (Agence Nationale de la Recherche) project (ANR-13-ADAP-0002) and by a travelling fellowships grant of the Company of Biologists sponsored by Journal of Experimental Biology (JEBTF-150809).

Adamo
,
S. A.
(
2012
).
The strings of the puppet master: how parasites change host behavior
. In
Parasitic Manipulation
(ed.
D.
Hughes
and
F.
Thomas
), pp.
36
-
51
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Alaux
,
C.
,
Ducloz
,
F.
,
Crauser
,
D.
and
Le Conte
,
Y.
(
2010
).
Diet effects on honeybee immunocompetence
.
Biol. Lett.
6
,
562
-
565
.
Alaux
,
C.
,
Crauser
,
D.
,
Pioz
,
M.
,
Saulnier
,
C.
and
Le Conte
,
Y.
(
2014
).
Parasitic and immune-modulation of flight activity in honey bees tracked with optical counters
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
3416
-
3424
.
Ardia
,
D. R.
,
Gantz
,
J. E.
,
Schneider
,
B. C.
and
Strebel
,
S.
(
2012
).
Costs of immunity in insects: an induced immune response increases metabolic rate and decreases antimicrobial activity
.
Funct. Ecol.
26
,
732
-
739
.
Bartholomew
,
G. A.
and
Casey
,
T. M.
(
1978
).
Oxygen consumption of moths during rest, pre-flight warm-up, and flight in relation to body size and wing morphology
.
J. Exp. Biol.
76
,
11
-
25
.
Biron
,
D. G.
and
Loxdale
,
H. D.
(
2013
).
Host-parasite molecular cross-talk during the manipulative process of a host by its parasite
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
148
-
160
.
Blaschon
,
B.
,
Guttenberger
,
H.
,
Hrassnig
,
N.
and
Crailsheim
,
K.
(
1999
).
Impact of bad weather on the development of the brood nest and pollen stores in a honeybee colony (Hymenoptera: Apidae)
.
Entomol. Gen.
24
,
49
-
60
.
Bordier
,
C.
,
Suchail
,
S.
,
Pioz
,
M.
,
Devaud
,
J.-M.
,
Collet
,
C.
,
Charreton
,
M.
,
Le Conte
,
Y.
and
Alaux
,
C.
(
2017a
).
Stress response in honeybees is associated with changes in task-related physiology and energetic metabolism
.
J. Insect Physiol.
98
,
47
-
54
.
Bordier
,
C.
,
Dechatre
,
H.
,
Suchail
,
S.
,
Peruzzi
,
M.
,
Soubeyrand
,
S.
,
Pioz
,
M.
,
Pélissier
,
M.
,
Crauser
,
D.
,
Conte
,
Y. L.
and
Alaux
,
C.
(
2017b
).
Colony adaptive response to simulated heat waves and consequences at the individual level in honeybees (Apis mellifera)
.
Sci. Rep.
7
,
3760
.
Brockmann
,
A.
,
Annangudi
,
S. P.
,
Richmond
,
T. A.
,
Ament
,
S. A.
,
Xie
,
F.
,
Southey
,
B. R.
,
Rodriguez-Zas
,
S. R.
,
Robinson
,
G. E.
and
Sweedler
,
J. V.
(
2009
).
Quantitative peptidomics reveal brain peptide signatures of behavior
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
106
,
2383
-
2388
.
Brodschneider
,
R.
and
Crailsheim
,
K.
(
2010
).
Nutrition and health in honey bees
.
Apidologie
41
,
278
-
294
.
Burnham
,
K. P.
and
Anderson
,
D. R.
(
2004
).
Multimodel inference: understanding AIC and BIC in model selection
.
Sociol. Methods Res.
33
,
261
-
304
.
Chen
,
Y. L.
,
Hung
,
Y. S.
and
Yang
,
E. C.
(
2008
).
Biogenic amine levels change in the brains of stressed honeybees
.
Arch. Insect Biochem. Physiol.
68
,
241
-
250
.
DeGrandi-Hoffman
,
G.
,
Chen
,
Y.
,
Huang
,
E.
and
Huang
,
M. H.
(
2010
).
The effect of diet on protein concentration, hypopharyngeal gland development and virus load in worker honey bees (Apis mellifera L.)
.
J. Insect Physiol.
56
,
1184
-
1191
.
Di Pasquale
,
G.
,
Salignon
,
M.
,
Le Conte
,
Y.
,
Belzunces
,
L. P.
,
Decourtye
,
A.
,
Kretzschmar
,
A.
,
Suchail
,
S.
,
Brunet
,
J. L.
and
Alaux
,
C.
(
2013
).
Influence of pollen nutrition on honey bee health: do pollen quality and diversity matter?
PLoS One
8
,
e72016
.
Dudley
,
R.
(
2000
).
The Biomechanics of Insect Flight: Form, Function, Evolution.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Evans
,
J. D.
,
Aronstein
,
K.
,
Chen
,
Y. P.
,
Hetru
,
C.
,
Imler
,
J.-L.
,
Jiang
,
H.
,
Kanost
,
M.
,
Thompson
,
G. J.
,
Zou
,
Z.
and
Hultmark
,
D.
(
2006
).
Immune pathways and defence mechanisms in honey bees Apis mellifera
.
Insect Mol. Biol.
15
,
645
-
656
.
Feltham
,
H.
,
Park
,
K.
and
Goulson
,
D.
(
2014
).
Field realistic doses of pesticide imidacloprid reduce bumblebee pollen foraging efficiency
.
Ecotoxicology
23
,
317
-
323
.
Feuerbacher
,
E.
,
Fewell
,
J. H.
,
Roberts
,
S. P.
,
Smith
,
E. F.
and
Harrison
,
J. F.
(
2003
).
Effects of load type (pollen or nectar) and load mass on hovering metabolic rate and mechanical power output in the honey bee Apis mellifera
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
1855
-
1865
.
Freitak
,
D.
,
Ots
,
I.
,
Vanatoa
,
A.
and
Hõrak
,
P.
and (
2003
).
Immune response is energetically costly in white cabbage butterfly pupae
.
Proc. R. Soc. B-Biological Sci.
270
,
S220
-
S222
.
Fussnecker
,
B. L.
,
Smith
,
B. H.
and
Mustard
,
J. A.
(
2006
).
Octopamine and tyramine influence the behavioral profile of locomotor activity in the honey bee (Apis mellifera)
.
J. Insect Physiol.
52
,
1083
-
1092
.
Gill
,
R. J.
and
Raine
,
N. E.
(
2014
).
Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposure
.
Funct. Ecol.
28
,
1459
-
1471
.
Goulson
,
D.
,
Nicholls
,
E.
,
Botías
,
C.
and
Rotheray
,
E. L.
(
2015
).
Bee declines driven by combined stress from parasites, pesticides, and lack of flowers
.
SciencExpress
2010
,
1
-
16
.
Jaumann
,
S.
,
Scudelari
,
R.
and
Naug
,
D.
(
2013
).
Energetic cost of learning and memory can cause cognitive impairment in honeybees
.
Biol. Lett.
9
,
20130149
.
Johnson
,
E. C.
and
White
,
M. P.
(
2009
).
Stressed-out insects: Hormonal actions and behavioral modifications
.
Horm. Brain Behav. Online
1069
-
1097
.
Klein
,
S.
,
Cabirol
,
A.
,
Devaud
,
J.-M.
,
Barron
,
A. B.
and
Lihoreau
,
M.
(
2017
).
Why bees are so vulnerable to environmental stressors
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
2201
,
1
-
11
.
Kralj
,
J.
and
Fuchs
,
S.
(
2010
).
Nosema sp. influences flight behavior of infected honey bee (Apis mellifera) foragers
.
Apidologie
41
,
21
-
28
.
Lach
,
L.
,
Kratz
,
M.
and
Baer
,
B.
(
2015
).
Parasitized honey bees are less likely to forage and carry less pollen
.
J. Invertebr. Pathol.
130
,
64
-
71
.
McDonnell
,
C. M.
,
Alaux
,
C.
,
Parrinello
,
H.
,
Desvignes
,
J.-P.
,
Crauser
,
D.
,
Durbesson
,
E.
,
Beslay
,
D.
and
Le Conte
,
Y.
(
2013
).
Ecto- and endoparasite induce similar chemical and brain neurogenomic responses in the honey bee (Apis mellifera)
.
BMC Ecol.
13
,
25
.
Naug
,
D.
(
2014
).
Infected honeybee foragers incur a higher loss in efficiency than in the rate of energetic gain
.
Biol. Lett.
10
,
20140731
.
Pankiw
,
T.
and
Page
,
R. E.
Jr
(
2000
).
Response thresholds to sucrose predict foraging division of labor in honeybees
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
47
,
265
-
267
.
Pankiw
,
T.
and
Page
,
R. E.
(
2003
).
Effect of pheromones, hormones, and handling on sucrose response thresholds of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sensory, Neural, Behav. Physiol.
189
,
675
-
684
.
Pernal
,
S. F.
and
Currie
,
R. W.
(
2000
).
Pollen quality of fresh and 1-year-old single pollen diets for worker honey bees (Apis mellifera L.)
.
Apidologie
31
,
387
-
409
.
Perry
,
C. J.
,
Søvik
,
E.
,
Myerscough
,
M. R.
and
Barron
,
A. B.
(
2015
).
Rapid behavioral maturation accelerates failure of stressed honey bee colonies
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
112
,
3427
-
3432
.
Potts
,
S. G.
,
Biesmeijer
,
J. C.
,
Kremen
,
C.
,
Neumann
,
P.
,
Schweiger
,
O.
and
Kunin
,
W. E.
(
2010
).
Global pollinator declines: Trends, impacts and drivers
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
25
,
345
-
353
.
Scheiner
,
R.
,
Plückhahn
,
S.
,
Öney
,
B.
,
Blenau
,
W.
and
Erber
,
J.
(
2002
).
Behavioural pharmacology of octopamine, tyramine and dopamine in honey bees
.
Behav. Brain Res.
136
,
545
-
553
.
Scheiner
,
R.
,
Toteva
,
A.
,
Reim
,
T.
,
Søvik
,
E.
and
Barron
,
A. B.
(
2014
).
Differences in the phototaxis of pollen and nectar foraging honey bees are related to their octopamine brain titers
.
Front. Physiol.
5
,
116
.
Schmickl
,
T.
and
Crailsheim
,
K.
(
2001
).
Cannibalism and early capping: Strategy of honeybee colonies in times of experimental pollen shortages
.
J. Comp. Physiol. - A Sensory, Neural, Behav. Physiol.
187
,
541
-
547
.
Schulz
,
D. J.
and
Robinson
,
G. E.
(
2001
).
Octopamine influences division of labor in honey bee colonies
.
J. Comp. Physiol. - A Sensory, Neural, Behav. Physiol.
187
,
53
-
61
.
Schulz
,
D. J.
,
Barron
,
A. B.
and
Robinson
,
G. E.
(
2002
).
A role for octopamine in honey bee division of labor
.
Brain. Behav. Evol.
60
,
350
-
359
.
Scofield
,
H. N.
and
Mattila
,
H. R.
(
2015
).
Honey bee workers that are pollen stressed as larvae become poor foragers and waggle dancers as adults
.
PLoS One
10
,
e0121731
.
Siede
,
R.
,
Meixner
,
M. D.
and
Büchler
,
R.
(
2012
).
Comparison of transcriptional changes of immune genes to experimental challenge in the honey bee (Apis mellifera)
.
J. Apic. Res.
51
,
320
-
328
.
Søvik
,
E.
,
Cornish
,
J. L.
and
Barron
,
A. B.
(
2013
).
Cocaine tolerance in honey bees
.
PLoS One
8
,
e64920
.
Søvik
,
E.
,
Perry
,
C. J.
,
LaMora
,
A.
,
Barron
,
A. B.
and
Ben-Shahar
,
Y.
(
2015
).
Negative impact of manganese on honeybee foraging
.
Biol. Lett.
11
,
20140989-20140989
.
Therneau
,
T. M.
and
Lumley
,
T.
(
2014
). Package “survival”. Survival Analysis. R package version 2.37-7
.
Wahl
,
O.
and
Ulm
,
K.
(
1983
).
Influence of pollen feeding and physiological condition on pesticide sensitivity of the honey bee Apis mellifera carnica
.
Oecologia
59
,
106
-
128
.
Winston
,
M. L.
(
1987
).
The Biology of the Honey Bee
.
Cambridge, MA ,USA
:
Harvard University Press
.
Wolf
,
S.
,
McMahon
,
D. P.
,
Lim
,
K. S.
,
Pull
,
C. D.
,
Clark
,
S. J.
,
Paxton
,
R. J.
and
Osborne
,
J. L.
(
2014
).
So near and yet so far: Harmonic radar reveals reduced homing ability of nosema infected honeybees
.
PLoS One
9
,
e103989
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.