The dive response, a decrease in heart rate (fH) and peripheral vasoconstriction, is the key mechanism allowing breath-hold divers to perform long-duration dives. This pronounced cardiovascular response to diving has been investigated intensely in pinnipeds, but comparatively little is known for cetaceans, in particular in ecologically relevant settings. Here, we studied the dive fH response in one of the smallest cetaceans, the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). We used a novel multi-sensor data logger to record dive behaviour, fH, ventilations and feeding events in three trained porpoises, providing the first evaluation of cetacean fH regulation while performing a variety of natural behaviours, including prey capture. We predicted that tagged harbour porpoises would exhibit a decrease in fH in all dives, but the degree of bradycardia would be influenced by dive duration and activity, i.e. the dive fH response would be exercise modulated. In all dives, fH decreased compared with surface rates by at least 50% (mean maximum surface fH=173 beats min−1, mean minimum dive fH=50 beats min−1); however, dive fH was approximately 10 beats min−1 higher in active dives as a result of a slower decrease in fH and more variable fH during pursuit of prey. We show that porpoises exhibit the typical breath-hold diver bradycardia during aerobic dives and that the fH response is modulated by exercise and dive duration; however, other variables such as expectations and individual differences are equally important in determining diving fH.

Successful and efficient foraging is essential for an organism's survival and reproduction. Marine mammals face an extraordinary foraging challenge compared with their terrestrial relatives in that they must exercise to search, capture and consume prey while holding their breath during often long and deep dives. Understanding the physiological mechanisms that underlie their ability to perform such extended breath-holds while transiting and foraging is essential if we are to understand their role in the ecosystem and their ability to exploit prey resources in a changing world.

The dive capacity of breath-hold divers is dependent on both the available O2 stores and the rate at which the O2 stores are depleted. Heart rate (fH), peripheral blood flow distribution and muscle workload are the primary determinants of the rate and pattern of O2 store use and ultimately breath-hold duration (Ponganis, 2015). Although recent advances in technology have greatly improved our understanding of O2 management in pinnipeds (seals and sea lions) (Hindle et al., 2010; McDonald and Ponganis, 2013, 2014; Meir et al., 2009), much less is known about how cetaceans (whales and dolphins) perform extended breath-hold dives (Elsner et al., 1966; Houser et al., 2010; Noren et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2015, 1999). To address this knowledge gap, we investigated the relationship between fine-scale diving behaviour, including feeding events, and fH in captive harbour porpoises, Phocoena phocoena (Linnaeus 1758).

Since the initial forced submersion studies (e.g. Scholander, 1940), decreased fH has been considered central to the diving ability of breath-hold divers. Cardiovascular regulation is critical during diving because changes in fH and blood flow affect not only blood O2 uptake from the lung but also the rate and magnitude of blood O2 delivery to tissues. During forced submersion, a severe bradycardia results in the isolation of muscle and peripheral organs from the blood flow, thereby slowing the depletion of blood O2, which is reserved for the heart and brain (Scholander et al., 1942; Zapol et al., 1979). More recent studies on trained and freely diving animals indicate that the dive response is dynamic, with declines in fH of varying degrees depending on dive duration and depth, exercise and expectations (Davis and Williams, 2012; Elmegaard et al., 2016; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Thompson and Fedak, 1993; Williams et al., 2015).

The advent of miniaturized data loggers has greatly improved our capability to measure and hence understand how wild pinnipeds manage O2, but because of the difficulty of deploying physiological loggers on wild cetaceans, most of what we know is from captive studies, primarily with bottlenose dolphins. Dolphins trained to dive to depths greater than 60 m exhibit a diving fH profile similar to profiles observed in diving seals and sea lions, decreasing from pre-dive values between 100 and 120 beats min−1 to lows of 20–30 beats min−1, before increasing again as the dolphin starts its ascent (Houser et al., 2010; Williams et al., 1999). Williams et al. (1999) concluded that the dive response dominates the typical mammalian exercise response (increase in fH and blood flow) because the degree of bradycardia observed in the diving dolphin was similar to values observed for shallow stationary dives (Elsner et al., 1966). However, more recently, a correlation between stroke rate and fH has been documented in short shallow dives of trained bottlenose dolphins (Davis and Williams, 2012; Noren et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2015). The authors proposed that this is because marine mammals exhibit an exercise-modulated fH response that maximizes the aerobic duration of a dive (Davis and Williams, 2012). Compared with bottlenose dolphins, a much less intense decline was seen in captive harbour porpoises during short shallow dives (Reed et al., 2000; Teilmann et al., 2006). Recently, porpoises were documented to alter the degree of bradycardia based on anticipated breath-hold duration (Elmegaard et al., 2016), but nothing is known about how they regulate fH in dives of different activity levels and durations.

From a diving physiology perspective, porpoises are interesting as they are among the smallest cetaceans and yet they live in low-temperature environments at high latitudes where elevated metabolic rates are needed to combat heat loss (Williams and Maresh, 2015). Wild porpoises meet these metabolic demands by high-rate foraging (Wisniewska et al., 2016). It is therefore essential to understand how they manage O2 in these active foraging dives if we are to understand how their physiological capacity may limit their ability to deal with natural and anthropogenic disturbances to their environment. Pilot whales have been documented to sprint at speeds of 3–9 m s−1 when actively foraging (Aguilar Soto et al., 2008), and it was hypothesized that during these feeding sprints, pilot whales may exhibit fH approaching maximum levels (fH,max) (Noren et al., 2012). Although porpoises do not exhibit such high speeds, they do increase activity during feeding events, allowing us to test this hypothesis with porpoises feeding on dead and live fish in captivity.

Here, we examined the dive response in three harbour porpoises using newly developed multi-sensor electrocardiogram (ECG) recording tags during stationary, active and prey capture dives. We investigated the relationship between fH and activity to assess the potential level of muscle perfusion during dives with different activity levels. We hypothesized that: (1) porpoises will decrease fH in all dives, but will only exhibit an extreme decrease in long-duration dives; (2) in more-typical short dives, harbour porpoises will show a positive relationship between fH and workload/activity, suggesting they are perfusing muscle during diving; and (3) fH will be elevated in prey capture sprints. Using a unique combination of controlled and free dives, we document the influence of both exercise and dive duration on the dive response. However, other factors such as individual differences and expectations are also important.

Animals

Dive fH was examined in three trained harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) at the Fjord & Belt Aquarium (Kerteminde, Denmark) from March to June 2014. Two of the porpoises were female, Freja (17 years old, 158 cm) and Sif (10 years old, 160 cm), and the third porpoise, Eigil, was male (17 years old, 151 cm). The porpoises were incidentally captured in pound nets as yearlings, and have been housed in a large net pen (35×10×5 m L×W×D) at the Fjord & Belt Aquarium since capture. Sif was 6–8 months pregnant during the study. The porpoises were housed at the Fjord & Belt Aquarium under permits from the Ministry of Environment and Food of Denmark (SN 343/FY-0014 and 1996-3446-0021). The experimental procedure was approved by the Aarhus University IACUC committee and complied with recommendations of the Danish Council for Experiments on Animals.

Instrumentation

A modified Dtag 3 (ECG-Dtag3, 15.5×8.5×3 cm, 265 g), a multi-sensor archival tag, was attached ∼5–10 cm behind the blowhole via four 5 cm silicone suction cups (Fig. 1A) (Johnson and Tyack, 2003). The ECG-Dtag 3 has two external silver chloride electrodes (10 mm) embedded in 5 cm suction cups that were placed rostral and caudal to the heart, close to the axis of the ventricle contraction to maximize the signal (Elmegaard et al., 2016). The ECG-Dtag3 recorded the differential potential between the electrodes relative to the ground in water sampling at 10 kHz (16-bit resolution and a 2-pole, 4 kHz anti-alias filter). Additionally, the tag recorded pressure and 3-axis acceleration at 2 kHz, and stereo sound (500 kHz sampling rate, 16 bit, 0.5–150 kHz bandwidth), allowing determination of activity, ventilations, echolocation behaviour and prey capture with precise synchrony to the fH data.

Fig. 1.

Instrumentation and experimentalprotocol. (A) A harbour porpoise instrumented with the ECG-Dtag 3 with two external electrodes. (B) Diagram of the experimental design with sample sizes (in parentheses) for each dive type and porpoise. Grey indicates the male porpoise, white represents the two female porpoises.

Fig. 1.

Instrumentation and experimentalprotocol. (A) A harbour porpoise instrumented with the ECG-Dtag 3 with two external electrodes. (B) Diagram of the experimental design with sample sizes (in parentheses) for each dive type and porpoise. Grey indicates the male porpoise, white represents the two female porpoises.

Experimental protocol

fH was collected using two protocols: (1) experimental and (2) opportunistic (Fig. 1B). For the experimental protocol, porpoises were trained to perform stationary and prey capture dives (with either dead or live fish) in a range of dive durations (∼0.5–2.5 min) while instrumented with the ECG-Dtag3. Experimental sessions consisted of a block of approximately 5–7 dives of the same dive category (stationary, dead prey capture or live prey capture). Dive duration was randomly assigned to each dive before the session with a goal of two short duration (∼30 s), two medium duration (∼45–55 s) and two long duration (>60 s) dives per session. Before and after each experimental dive, the porpoise stationed for a minimum of 1 min at the surface with minimal activity to make sure the blood gases reached resting levels. This ensured that each dive was independent of the proceeding dive. The 1 min duration was selected after review of fH and respiration rate data obtained during training sessions. All sessions were recorded with a GoPro mounted on the observer's head for validation of tag-collected behavioural data.

For stationary dives, porpoises stationed on a bite plate (Freja) or target (Sif, Eigil) at 1 m depth. Water movement in the sea pen varies with tide phase and so the target was oriented to minimize the effort required to stay on station. The porpoises remained on station until a whistle signalled that the behaviour was complete. Stationary dives were between ∼30 and 70 s in duration. If dives were extended much beyond 60 s, the porpoise would return to the surface before the trainer recalled the animal. For the dead prey capture dives, a trainer was located at one end of pool with the porpoise and a ‘feeder’ was located at the opposite side (∼20 m apart). When the trainer sent the porpoise on a trial, the feeder splashed and tossed a fish into the water (capelin or herring). Once the porpoise caught the fish, another fish was tossed into the pool a few metres away, and this continued until the animal was recalled or, on longer dives, surfaced on its own. Dive duration was determined by varying the number of fish, i.e. 1 fish for short dives, 3–5 fish for medium duration dives, and for long duration dives, we provided fish until the animal surfaced. For the long duration dives, the 1 min surface interval started with the first breath after the porpoise returned to the trainer. The live prey capture dives were conducted exactly like the dead prey capture trials, except live black gobies (Gobius niger – a local species that the porpoises occasionally feed on when they enter the net pen) were used rather than dead fish.

In addition to the experimental sessions, we opportunistically collected data during (1) free swims between sessions and (2) training sessions where the porpoises had silicone eye cups placed over their eyes as they performed short to medium duration dead prey capture trials as described above (eye cup prey capture). The free swims allowed us to examine fH during dives where the porpoises controlled the dive duration, activity and surface duration.

Data processing and analyses

All data processing and analyses were performed using custom-written scripts in MatLab (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Sound files were examined aurally and visually in 5 s windows using a spectrogram display (Hamming window, fast Fourier transform size 512, 75% overlap). Ventilations and feeding buzzes were identified (Deruiter et al., 2009; Wisniewska et al., 2016). Dives were determined from the ventilations: the start of the dive was the time of the last ventilation before the porpoise initiated the dive; the end of the dive was the first ventilation after the completion of the behaviour. This usually coincided with the porpoise returning to the trainer, but on longer dives, the end of dive ventilation often occurred before the porpoise was recalled to the trainer. In either case, the post-dive interval did not begin until the porpoise returned to the trainer. In addition to identifying dives, we also estimated bottom time. Because the dives were shallow and varied in depth as the porpoises chased fish, the bottom of the dive was not always clear from the dive profiles. Therefore, bottom time was estimated to begin 5 s after the dive start and end 5 s before dive end. This eliminates the periods of rapid decline and the anticipatory increase in fH at the beginning and end of each dive, respectively.

ECG data were decimated to a sampling rate of 250 Hz, and filtered to remove excess noise [fir filter to remove 50 Hz noise, followed by Butterworth filters (0.1, 0.3)]. R-peaks were identified using a custom-written peak detector script and all records were visually inspected to correct for missing or spurious peaks. Instantaneous fH was determined from the R–R peak intervals.

We calculated two activity indexes from the raw 3-axis acceleration data: (1) minimum specific acceleration (MSA), a measure of how much the total acceleration deviates from the gravity acceleration (Simon et al., 2012) – this is an underestimate of the specific acceleration generated by the animal and is calculated as the absolute value of the norm of acceleration minus the gravity acceleration; and (2) overall dynamic body acceleration (ODBA), another integrated measurement of body motion in three spatial dimensions, which is the norm of the high-pass-filtered acceleration (Wilson et al., 2006). MSA and ODBA were highly correlated (see General results). For this reason, only MSA was used to investigate the relationship between activity and fH.

A suite of fH variables was determined for each dive including: total number of heart beats during a dive (total heart beats), dive fH (total heart beats/dive duration), initial and minimum instantaneous dive fH, bottom fH (heart beats during bottom of dive/bottom time duration), decrease in instantaneous fH in the first 5 s of dive (fH at 5 s−initial fH); and for experimental dives with a 1 min surface interval between dives, we calculated pre- and post-dive fH (number of heart beats in the 30 s proceeding or following the dive/0.5 min) and pre- and post-dive mode (most common instantaneous fH within bin-sizes of ∼5 beats min−1). Additionally, for each dive we calculated average MSA (total MSA/dive duration).

To investigate the relationship between feeding behaviour and fH, average instantaneous fH and total MSA was calculated for a 10 s window centred at the start of every feeding buzz. For comparison, average instantaneous fH and total MSA were also calculated for a 10 s window at the beginning of each feeding dive (10–20 s into the dive).

The relationship between MSA and ODBA was evaluated by performing correlation analyses. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were used to investigate the relationship between dive behaviour and fH, with porpoise ID as a covariate to account for individual differences between porpoises (JMP, SAS, Cary, NC, USA). Differences in pre-dive fH, dive fH, minimum fH, post-dive fH, decline in fH in the first 5 s of a dive, and average dive MSA between the dive types were investigated using two-way ANOVA, followed by post hoc Tukey tests. In all models, dive type (stationary, prey capture, etc.) and individual (porpoise ID) were fixed effects. The influence of feeding behaviour on activity and fH was also investigated using two-way ANOVA to test for differences in fH and activity between buzz and non-buzz segments. Dive types (dead versus live prey capture) were analysed separately. Feeding state (buzz/non-buzz) and porpoise ID were the fixed variables. The relationships between dive duration and MSA to dive fH and minimum fH were investigated using ANCOVA with porpoise ID as a covariate. Residuals of the final models were evaluated to assess model violations. No assumptions were violated.

General results

fH and activity data were obtained from 437 dives from 3 porpoises (Fig. 1, Table 1). The porpoises exhibited a large range in fH from over 200 beats min−1 at the surface down to 12 beats min−1 in the longest dive of 4 min (Fig. 2). Dive MSA was highly correlated with ODBA (Pearson r=0.98, P>0.001). We therefore used dive MSA for all analyses investigating the relationship between fH and activity.

Table 1.

Heart rate (fH) and activity (MSA) summary statistics for experimental and opportunistic dives and for each porpoise

Heart rate (fH) and activity (MSA) summary statistics for experimental and opportunistic dives and for each porpoise
Heart rate (fH) and activity (MSA) summary statistics for experimental and opportunistic dives and for each porpoise
Fig. 2.

Diving heart rate (fH) profiles and minimum specific acceleration (MSA) from an adult female harbour porpoise (Freja: F2). (A–C) Examples of the systematically collected data: (A) a long stationary dive, (B) a dead prey capture dive, and (C) a live prey capture dive. (D–F) Examples of opportunistically collected data: (D) dead prey capture dives while the animal wears eye cups, (E) typical short free dives (not during a training session), and (F) the longest free dive recorded. Triangles indicate a breath (solid triangles indicate the breath before the start and after the end of a dive), grey shading indicates a dive, and red indicates when the porpoise produced a buzz.

Fig. 2.

Diving heart rate (fH) profiles and minimum specific acceleration (MSA) from an adult female harbour porpoise (Freja: F2). (A–C) Examples of the systematically collected data: (A) a long stationary dive, (B) a dead prey capture dive, and (C) a live prey capture dive. (D–F) Examples of opportunistically collected data: (D) dead prey capture dives while the animal wears eye cups, (E) typical short free dives (not during a training session), and (F) the longest free dive recorded. Triangles indicate a breath (solid triangles indicate the breath before the start and after the end of a dive), grey shading indicates a dive, and red indicates when the porpoise produced a buzz.

Surface fH and sinus arrhythmia

Pre-dive surface fH averaged 121±22 beats min−1, but there were significant differences between individuals, with Sif and Eigil having significantly higher fH (Sif: 133±16 beats min−1, Eigil: 132±17 beats min−1) compared with that for Freja (104±18 beats min−1) (Table S1). Pre-dive fH did not differ between stationary dives and dead prey capture dives, but was approximately 10 beats min−1 higher before live prey capture dives (Table 1). During the 1 min surface intervals, the porpoises often exhibited sinus arrhythmias with fH decreasing from over 150 beats min−1 to values resembling fH exhibited during dives; however, they did not show this decrease between every ventilation (Fig. 3A). During the free swim, we saw similar patterns and often if porpoises took two ventilation between dives, fH stayed elevated between these ventilations (Fig. 3B).

Fig. 3.

Surface fH and sinus arrhythmia. (A) The surface fH between experimental dives is variable, but a clear respiratory sinus arrhythmia is not present in this case. Surface fH may decrease to fH observed during a dive between breaths, but often remains high between breaths as observed in seals, sea lions and dolphins (Andrews et al., 1997; Houser et al., 2010; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014). (B) In the short surface intervals exhibited during free dives, fH remains elevated between breaths. Data from an adult female porpoise (Sif: F1). Grey shading indicates a dive.

Fig. 3.

Surface fH and sinus arrhythmia. (A) The surface fH between experimental dives is variable, but a clear respiratory sinus arrhythmia is not present in this case. Surface fH may decrease to fH observed during a dive between breaths, but often remains high between breaths as observed in seals, sea lions and dolphins (Andrews et al., 1997; Houser et al., 2010; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014). (B) In the short surface intervals exhibited during free dives, fH remains elevated between breaths. Data from an adult female porpoise (Sif: F1). Grey shading indicates a dive.

Dive fH and activity

Dive fH was 51±8% lower than the pre-dive fH (Fig. 2). Dive fH profiles, in general, were U-shaped with a rapid decline, a relatively stable fH near the bottom, and a rapid increase in fH as the animal ascended (Fig. 2). Within the first 5 s of the dive, fH decreased an average of 65–89 beats min−1, depending on dive type (Table 1). Although all porpoises exhibited a similar fH profile shape, they differed in surface and dive fH (Fig. 4B; Fig. S1, Table S1).

Fig. 4.

Dive fH and activity by dive type. (A) Activity (MSA) and (B) fH increase from low values in stationary dives to high values in live prey capture dives in all three porpoises, but the degree of change differs (Table 1). fH was lowest during prey capture dives with eye cups, despite activity levels being intermediate between those of dead and live prey capture dives (without eye cups). The highest dive fH was observed in free dives. Data are means±s.e.m., 437 dives from 3 porpoises.

Fig. 4.

Dive fH and activity by dive type. (A) Activity (MSA) and (B) fH increase from low values in stationary dives to high values in live prey capture dives in all three porpoises, but the degree of change differs (Table 1). fH was lowest during prey capture dives with eye cups, despite activity levels being intermediate between those of dead and live prey capture dives (without eye cups). The highest dive fH was observed in free dives. Data are means±s.e.m., 437 dives from 3 porpoises.

To investigate the influence of activity and duration on fH, porpoises performed dives of varying duration with significantly different activity levels as measured by the dive MSA (Fig. 4A, Table 1). Stationary dives had low activity (dive MSA=0.39±0.11 m s−2); activity increased approximately 80% during dead prey capture dives (dive MSA=0.71±0.15 m s−2) and an additional ∼45% in live prey capture dives (dive MSA=1.04±0.18 m s−2). Dive and minimum fH differed significantly across the experimental dive types, with the lowest fH exhibited during stationary dives and the highest fH in live prey capture dives (Figs 2 and 4, Table 1). Additionally, post-dive fH following dead and live prey capture dives was significantly higher than that following stationary dives (Table 1).

Relationship between fH and activity

Activity and dive duration both influenced fH in the experimental dives when porpoises did not know how long they would be requested to dive [Table 2; fH=59.9+22.7(MSA)−0.14(duration)+(ID correction: Eigil=9.6, Sif=1.1, Freja=−10.7), R2=0.74, F4,316=222.4, P<0.0001]. Dive duration had a significant, but small, negative effect on dive fH when controlling for MSA (Fig. 5A, Table 2), while activity had a positive effect on fH (Fig. 5B, Table 2). The non-significant interaction terms (duration×ID and MSA×ID) indicate that the relationship of dive duration and activity to dive fH was the same for all three porpoises (same slope), although they had significantly different intercepts. During free dives, when porpoises controlled activity level and dive duration, fH decreased with increasing dive duration for all porpoises, while activity did not have an impact on dive fH (Fig. 5C,D, Table 2). However, the significant duration×ID interaction term indicates that the relationship was not consistent between porpoises (Table 2). In prey capture dives when the porpoises had silicone eye cups placed over their eyes, dive and minimum fH were lower than values during stationary dives, despite activity levels similar to prey capture dives without eye cups (Fig. 2, Table 1).

Table 2.

Parameter estimates and confidence intervals for ANCOVA examining the relationship of dive duration and activity to fHin experimental and free dives

Parameter estimates and confidence intervals for ANCOVA examining the relationship of dive duration and activity to fH in experimental and free dives
Parameter estimates and confidence intervals for ANCOVA examining the relationship of dive duration and activity to fH in experimental and free dives
Fig. 5.

Relationship of dive duration and activity (MSA) to dive fH. (A,B) During experimental dives, there was a positive relationship between activity and dive fH (B) and a small negative relationship between dive duration and dive fH (A) [Table 2: fH=59.9+22.7(MSA)−0.14(duration)+(ID correction), R2=0.74). (C,D) In free dives, there was a negative relationship between dive duration and dive fH (C), but no relationship between activity and fH (D) (Table 2). Data represent 407 dives from 3 porpoises.

Fig. 5.

Relationship of dive duration and activity (MSA) to dive fH. (A,B) During experimental dives, there was a positive relationship between activity and dive fH (B) and a small negative relationship between dive duration and dive fH (A) [Table 2: fH=59.9+22.7(MSA)−0.14(duration)+(ID correction), R2=0.74). (C,D) In free dives, there was a negative relationship between dive duration and dive fH (C), but no relationship between activity and fH (D) (Table 2). Data represent 407 dives from 3 porpoises.

In addition to examining the relationship between fH and activity at the level of the dive, we also investigated fH during feeding buzzes, which are often associated with a burst of activity. The accelerometry data support the hypothesis that there is increased activity associated with a feeding event, with a 30–50% increase in total MSA during the 10 s window centred at the start of the buzz compared with that at a 10 s window at the beginning of the dive with no feeding events (Fig. 6, Table 3). Despite this increase in activity, there was a small (5%) or no increase in fH (Fig. 6, Table 3). However, overall activity was higher during live prey capture dives, which was associated with higher fH in live prey capture dives (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6.

Activity (MSA) and fH during a 10 s window early in the dive (10–20 s) before feeding events, and during feeding (centred on the start of buzz). All porpoises increased activity during feeding events (both dead and live prey) (A), but fH did not increase to match this increased activity, except for Eigil (M1) during dead prey capture trials (B). In contrast, Freja (F2) showed a decrease in fH during live prey capture events despite an increase in activity. Data are means±s.e.m., sample size is indicate in the bars.

Fig. 6.

Activity (MSA) and fH during a 10 s window early in the dive (10–20 s) before feeding events, and during feeding (centred on the start of buzz). All porpoises increased activity during feeding events (both dead and live prey) (A), but fH did not increase to match this increased activity, except for Eigil (M1) during dead prey capture trials (B). In contrast, Freja (F2) showed a decrease in fH during live prey capture events despite an increase in activity. Data are means±s.e.m., sample size is indicate in the bars.

Table 3.

Effect tests results from comparison of fH and activity (MSA) during 10 s windows centred on a feeding buzz and 10 s window with no buzzes

Effect tests results from comparison of fH and activity (MSA) during 10 s windows centred on a feeding buzz and 10 s window with no buzzes
Effect tests results from comparison of fH and activity (MSA) during 10 s windows centred on a feeding buzz and 10 s window with no buzzes

Dive and surface fH

Little is known about fH management in cetaceans during ecologically relevant activities such as foraging because of the difficulty associated with obtaining such data from free-ranging animals. Here, we investigated fH in captive harbour porpoises, a model species, as they performed a variety of dive types, including foraging dives. The U-shaped dive fH profile exhibited by harbour porpoises is akin to fH profiles observed in aerobic dives of seals, sea lions and captive dolphins (Davis and Williams, 2012; Hill et al., 1987; Hindle et al., 2010; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Thompson and Fedak, 1993). A previous study on fH in harbour porpoises performing surface breath-holds documented a progressive development of bradycardia over the duration of the breath-hold (Reed et al., 2000). Reed et al. (2000) proposed that this was because harbour porpoises continue to use the lungs as an O2 store while diving. Because harbour porpoises are relatively shallow divers in comparison to many other cetaceans, the air in the lungs (containing ∼80% nitrogen) may be less of a liability than in deeper diving species. While lung O2 stores may be an important source of O2 in these shallow divers, in our study they exhibited a rapid decrease in fH like other breath-hold divers, disproving the hypothesis that bradycardia onset is slow as a result of the importance of lung O2 stores.

As we predicted, porpoises exhibited lower fH in longer duration dives, as observed in freely diving seals, sea lions and penguins (Hill et al., 1987; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Thompson and Fedak, 1993; Wright et al., 2014). The relationship was weak in experimental dives with randomized dive durations, probably because the porpoises could not predict dive duration. However, during free swims when porpoises controlled dive duration, there was a strong negative relationship between dive duration and fH. During short free dives (<20 s), fH only decreased to around 50–100 beats min−1, while in the longest dive (4 min), fH decreased to 12 beats min−1, and stayed below 15 beats min−1 for over 1 min. We only observed one dive >2.5 min, and the level of bradycardia in this 4 min dive was lower than that typically observed in 4 min dives of California sea lions (McDonald and Ponganis, 2014) and most other pinnipeds (Fedak et al., 1988; Thompson and Fedak, 1993), indicating that although most dives of harbour porpoises are short and shallow, they are capable of planning and executing extreme bradycardia if needed to extend dive duration.

Although the fH profile shape was similar to that of other breath-hold divers, the surface respiratory fH documented here is the highest reported for any adult marine mammal (Boyd et al., 1999; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Thompson and Fedak, 1993; Williams et al., 2015). Although mean fH during surface resting varied between individuals, all three porpoises exhibited instantaneous fH of over 200 beats min−1 before 8–23% and after 8–41% of their dives. Mean maximum pre-dive fH ranged from 156 to 191 beats min−1. In comparison, bottlenose dolphin calves, juveniles and adults had a mean maximum surface fH of ∼120–130 beats min−1 (Noren et al., 2004) and adults had maximum exercising  fH of ∼150 beats min−1 (Williams et al., 1993). Only juvenile sea lions, which are approximately 20–30 kg lighter than porpoises, have been reported with respiratory fH as high as that of the porpoises in this study (Ponganis et al., 1997). Although it is inherently difficult to define the resting fH in a cetacean that has irregular breathing and is diving continuously, from scaling relationships the predicted fH for a 50 kg mammal is approximately 55 beats min−1 (White and Kearney, 2014). It is interesting to note that this is similar to the fH found here for porpoises performing a stationary dive at 1 m depth, but such a comparison may be misleading, as it represents a mixture of a high metabolic rates (typical of marine mammals) countered by a dive response.

The combination of a high surface fH and a moderate dive bradycardia supports the hypothesis that porpoises have high metabolic rates which will limit their dive durations. The calculated aerobic dive limit (cADL) of porpoises has been estimated to be between 3 and 5.4 min, depending on the metabolic rate used in the calculation (Noren et al., 2014; Reed et al., 2000). Based on the high fH observed in this study we predict that the true ADL will be on the lower end of that range. This is consistent with studies on wild porpoises that found that 94–95% of dives are <3 min in duration (Otani et al., 1998; Westgate et al., 1995).

Exercise modulated fH response

Currently, one of the biggest knowledge gaps in the diving physiology of marine mammals relates to muscle perfusion during natural dives. Although this is difficult to measure directly, the role of muscle perfusion during diving is often evaluated by examining the relationship between fH and workload (activity). Porpoises exhibited an exercise-modulated fH response during experimental dives (Figs 4 and 5), as observed in Weddell seals and captive bottlenose dolphins (Davis and Williams, 2012; Noren et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2015). The positive relationship between activity (MSA) and fH suggests that muscle perfusion increases as a function of muscle workload in the short aerobic dives. The exercise-modulated dive response has been proposed to be key to maximizing aerobic dive duration (Davis and Williams, 2012; Williams et al., 2015). The decrease in fH during diving is needed to efficiently use blood and muscle O2 stores (Davis and Kanatous, 1999); however, the degree of bradycardia can be adjusted throughout dives to balance the O2 demands of exercising muscle (Davis and Williams, 2012). Although we found a relationship between activity and fH for porpoises, the relationship was much more subtle and variable than that observed in dolphins. Between stationary and active dives, there was only a ∼25% increase in dive fH despite an increase in activity, as measured by excess triaxial acceleration, of 160%. In comparison, there was a 39–57% increase in bottlenose dolphin fH between stationary dives and horizontal swimming dives (Noren et al., 2012), and within dives, there was almost a direct relationship between stroke rate and fH (Davis and Williams, 2012). The strong relationship between fH and activity in dolphins led Noren et al. (2012) to propose that some cetaceans, like pilot whales, may exhibit high fH during prey chases at depth (Aguilar Soto et al., 2008). Within the experimental limits of this study, we found no evidence for this in harbour porpoises. Although activity levels did transiently increase by 30–50% during a prey capture event, there was no increase in fH associated with prey capture. Because porpoises only exhibited a moderate bradycardia in the prey capture dives, and are probably therefore maintaining some muscle blood flow, they may not need to increase fH during the short bursts of activity. Alternatively, because of the suspected high metabolic rates, they may not increase fH during the bursts of activity because they need to conserve O2 for the brain and heart, even in these short dives.

Expectations and individual variability

The high variability in fH associated with overall dive activity (but not individual prey captures) in experimental dives, but the lack of a similar relationship during free dives, suggests that, although porpoises do exhibit an exercise-modulated dive response at times, other factors are likely to be important in determining the level of bradycardia. For example, fH was lower in dives in which porpoises performed prey captures with silicone cups over their eyes, ensuring they only used echolocation to locate prey. This suggests that in situations when they cannot use all their senses to evaluate their surroundings, they are more conservative with their O2 use, indicating volatile control over fH. This is further supported by the weak relationship between dive fH and dive duration when they do not know how long the dive task will be, in contrast to the strong relationship during free dives. When porpoises are unsure whether they will be asked to perform a short or long dive, they appear to manage O2 in a fashion that will allow them to successfully perform a long dive. Interestingly, in some long prey capture dives, fH was variable at the beginning of the dive, but then stabilized at a lower value when fish continued to be provided (Fig. 2). The ability to modify fH based on expectations has recently been documented in the same porpoises performing stationary dives (Elmegaard et al., 2016) and has been suggested for other species including sea lions and bottlenose dolphins (Elsner et al., 1966; Kooyman and Campbell, 1972; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Ridgway et al., 1975).

Although dive duration and activity influenced fH for all three porpoises, the porpoises had different surface and dive fH. Eigil (M1), the small male (151 cm, ∼45 kg), had both the highest surface fH and highest dive fH (Fig. 4; Table S1). Previous studies have also found smaller individuals tend to have higher fH (McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Williams et al., 2015). Sif (F1) and Freja (F2) were similar in size (160 and 158 cm, ∼60–65 kg) and exhibited similar stationary dive fH. However, Sif, the pregnant porpoise, exhibited surface fH that was much higher, approximating values exhibited by Eigil at the surface. Also, as her activity level increased, her fH increased during dives at a faster rate than that of Freja, resulting in dive fH equivalent to that of Eigil in live prey capture dives (Fig. 4, Table 1). The higher fH exhibited by Sif is probably due to pregnancy. In humans, cardiac output increases by 30–50% by late pregnancy, partly due to a ∼15–25% increase in fH (Hunter and Robson, 1992). Despite the differences in fH, all porpoises exhibited lower fH associated with longer dives and low activity.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we successfully obtained diving fH profiles from porpoises performing both controlled experimental and free dives. We found that fH decreases during all dives and that the fH profile shape resembled that of aerobic dives of other breath-hold divers. In many dives, dive fH did not differ appreciably from the fH exhibited during short breath-holds within surface intervals. Both dive duration and activity influenced fH, but the effect depended on whether porpoises were performing experimental or opportunistic dives. When the porpoises self-selected dive duration and activity during free dives, there was a strong relationship between dive duration and dive fH. This was much weaker during experimental dives, where we found a stronger relationship between fH and activity. This supports the hypothesis that during aerobic dives the dive response may be exercise modulated. As fH was rarely below 40 beats min−1, there was probably some muscle perfusion in most dives, and it increased slightly as the activity level increased. Although we found some support for the exercise-modulated dive response hypothesis, we did not find evidence of increases in fH associated with feeding sprints. Additionally, the high variability in fH and the differences observed between experimental and opportunistic dives indicate that other factors, such as expectation, influence dive fH.

We thank the trainers at Fjord & Belt Aquarium, particularly Jakob Kristensen and Josefin Larson, for exceptional support and Danuta Wisniewska for assistance with use of the Dtag3 and data analysis. Rene Swift is thanked for producing and testing the suction cup electrodes.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: B.I.M., P.T.M.; Methodology: B.I.M., M.J., P.T.M.; Formal analysis: B.I.M.; Investigation: B.I.M.; Resources: M.J., P.T.M.; Tag development: M.J.; Writing - original draft: B.I.M.; Writing - review & editing: B.I.M., M.J., P.T.M.; Supervision: P.T.M.; Project administration: B.I.M.; Funding acquisition: B.I.M., M.J., P.T.M.

Funding

This research was supported by a National Science Foundation International Research Postdoctoral Fellowship (OISE – 1159123 awarded to B.I.M.); and grants from the Office of Naval Research (N000141410416) and the Carlsbergfondet (awarded to P.T.M. and B.I.M.). M.J. was funded by a Marie Curie Sklodowska Career Integration Grant (FP7-PEOPLE-2011_CIG) and by the Marine Alliance for Science and Technology Scotland.

Data availability

Data are available from figshare digital repository: 10.6084/m9.figshare.5549329.

Aguilar Soto
,
N.
,
Johnson
,
M. P.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Díaz
,
F.
,
Domínguez
,
I.
,
Brito
,
A.
and
Tyack
,
P.
(
2008
).
Cheetahs of the deep sea: deep foraging sprints in short-finned pilot whales off Tenerife (Canary Islands)
.
J. Anim. Ecol.
77
,
936
-
947
.
Andrews
,
R. D.
,
Jones
,
D. R.
,
Williams
,
J. D.
,
Thorson
,
P. H.
,
Oliver
,
G. W.
,
Costa
,
D. P.
and
LeBoeuf
,
B. J.
(
1997
).
Heart rates of northern elephant seals diving at sea and resting on the beach
.
J. Exp. Biol.
200
,
2083
-
2095
.
Boyd
,
I.
,
Bevan
,
R.
,
Woakes
,
A.
and
Butler
,
P.
(
1999
).
Heart rate and behavior of fur seals: implications for measurement of field energetics
.
Am. J. Physiol.
276
,
H844
-
H857
.
Davis
,
R. W.
and
Kanatous
,
S. B.
(
1999
).
Convective oxygen transport and tissue oxygen consumption in Weddell seals during aerobic dives
.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
1091
-
1113
.
Davis
,
R. W.
and
Williams
,
T. M.
(
2012
).
The marine mammal dive response is exercise modulated to maximize aerobic dive duration
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
198
,
583
-
591
.
Deruiter
,
S. L.
,
Bahr
,
A.
,
Blanchet
,
M.-A.
,
Hansen
,
S. F.
,
Kristensen
,
J. H.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Tyack
,
P. L.
and
Wahlberg
,
M.
(
2009
).
Acoustic behaviour of echolocating porpoises during prey capture
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
3100
-
3107
.
Elmegaard
,
S. L.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
and
McDonald
,
B. I.
(
2016
).
Cognitive control of heart rate in diving harbor porpoises
.
Curr. Biol.
26
,
R1175
-
R1176
.
Elsner
,
R.
,
Kenney
,
D. W.
and
Burgess
,
K.
(
1966
).
Diving bradycardia in the trained dolphin
.
Nature
212
,
407
-
408
.
Fedak
,
M. A.
,
Pullen
,
M. R.
and
Kanwisher
,
J.
(
1988
).
Circulatory responses of seals to periodic breathing: heart rate and breathing during exercise and diving in the laboratory and open sea
.
Can. J. Zool.
66
,
53
-
60
.
Hill
,
R. D.
,
Schneider
,
R. C.
,
Liggins
,
G. C.
,
Schuette
,
A. H.
,
Elliott
,
R. L.
,
Guppy
,
M.
,
Hochachka
,
P. W.
,
Qvist
,
J.
,
Falke
,
K. J.
and
Zapol
,
W. M.
(
1987
).
Heart rate and body temperature during free diving of Weddell seals
.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
253
,
R344
-
R351
.
Hindle
,
A. G.
,
Young
,
B. L.
,
Rosen
,
D. A. S.
,
Haulena
,
M.
and
Trites
,
A. W.
(
2010
).
Dive response differs between shallow- and deep-diving Steller sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus)
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
394
,
141
-
148
.
Houser
,
D. S.
,
Dankiewicz-Talmadge
,
L. A.
,
Stockard
,
T. K.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2010
).
Investigation of the potential for vascular bubble formation in a repetitively diving dolphin
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
52
-
62
.
Hunter
,
S.
and
Robson
,
S. C.
(
1992
).
Adaptation of the maternal heart in pregnancy
.
Br. Heart J.
68
,
540
.
Johnson
,
M. P.
and
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
2003
).
A digital acoustic recording tag for measuring the response of wild marine mammals to sound
.
IEEE. J. Oceanic. Eng.
28
,
3
-
12
.
Kooyman
,
G. L.
and
Campbell
,
W. B.
(
1972
).
Heart rates in freely diving Weddell seals, Leptonychotes weddelli
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Physiol.
43
,
31
-
36
.
McDonald
,
B. I.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2013
).
Insights from venous oxygen profiles: oxygen utilization and management in diving California sea lions
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
3332
-
3341
.
McDonald
,
B. I.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2014
).
Deep-diving sea lions exhibit extreme bradycardia in long-duration dives
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
1525
-
1534
.
Meir
,
J. U.
,
Champagne
,
C. D.
,
Costa
,
D. P.
,
Williams
,
C. L.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2009
).
Extreme hypoxemic tolerance and blood oxygen depletion in diving elephant seals
.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
297
,
R927
-
R939
.
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Cuccurullo
,
V.
and
Williams
,
T. M.
(
2004
).
The development of diving bradycardia in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
174
,
139
-
147
.
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Kendall
,
T.
,
Cuccurullo
,
V.
and
Williams
,
T. M.
(
2012
).
The dive response redefined: underwater behavior influences cardiac variability in freely diving dolphins
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
2735
-
2741
.
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Noren
,
D. P.
and
Gaydos
,
J. K.
(
2014
).
Living in the fast lane: rapid development of the locomotor muscle in immature harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
184
,
1065
-
1076
.
Otani
,
S.
,
Naito
,
Y.
,
Kawamura
,
A.
,
Kawasaki
,
M.
,
Nishiwaki
,
S.
and
Kato
,
A.
(
1998
).
Diving behavior and performance of harbor porpoises, Phocoena phocoena, in Funka Bay, Hokkaido, Japan
.
Mar. Mamm. Sci.
14
,
209
-
220
.
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2015
).
Diving Physiology of Marine Mammals and Seabirds
.
Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
.
Ponganis
,
P. J.
,
Kooyman
,
G. L.
,
Winter
,
L. M.
and
Starke
,
L. N.
(
1997
).
Heart rate and plasma lactate responses during submerged swimming and trained diving in California sea lions, Zalophus californianus
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
167
,
9
-
16
.
Reed
,
J. Z.
,
Chambers
,
C.
,
Hunter
,
C. J.
,
Lockyer
,
C.
,
Kastelein
,
R.
,
Fedak
,
M. A.
and
Boutilier
,
R. G.
(
2000
).
Gas exchange and heart rate in the harbour porpoise, Phocoena phocoena
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
170
,
1
-
10
.
Ridgway
,
S. H.
,
Carder
,
D. A.
and
Clark
,
W.
(
1975
).
Conditioned bradycardia in the sea lion Zalophus californianus
.
Nature
256
,
37
-
38
.
Scholander
,
P. F.
(
1940
).
Experimental Investigations on the Respiratory Function in Diving Mammals and Birds
.
Hvalradets Skrifter
.
22
,
1
-
131
.
Scholander
,
P.
,
Irving
,
L.
and
Grinnell
,
S.
(
1942
).
Aerobic and anaerobic changes in seal muscles during diving
.
J. Biol. Chem.
142
,
431
-
440
.
Simon
,
M.
,
Johnson
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2012
).
Keeping momentum with a mouthful of water: behavior and kinematics of humpback whale lunge feeding
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
3786
-
3798
.
Teilmann
,
J.
,
Tougaard
,
J.
,
Miller
,
L. A.
,
Kirketerp
,
T.
,
Hansen
,
K.
and
Brando
,
S.
(
2006
).
Reactions of captive harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) to pinger-like sounds
.
Mar. Mamm. Sci.
22
,
240
-
260
.
Thompson
,
D.
and
Fedak
,
M. A.
(
1993
).
Cardiac responses of grey seals during diving at sea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
174
,
139
-
154
.
Westgate
,
A. J.
,
Head
,
A. J.
,
Berggren
,
P.
,
Koopman
,
H. N.
and
Gaskin
,
D. E.
(
1995
).
Diving behaviour of harbour porpoises, Phocoena phocoena
.
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci.
52
,
1064
-
1073
.
White
,
C. R.
and
Kearney
,
M. R.
(
2014
).
Metabolic scaling in animals: methods, empirical results, and theoretical explanations
.
Compr. Physiol.
4
,
231
-
256
.
Williams
,
T. M.
and
Maresh
,
J. L.
(
2015
).
Exercise energetics
. In
Marine Mammal Physiology. Requisites for Ocean Living
(ed.
M. A.
Castellini
and
J.-A.
Mellish
), pp.
47
-
68
.
Boca Raton
:
CRC Press
.
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Friedl
,
W. A.
and
Haun
,
J. E.
(
1993
).
The physiology of bottle-nosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) - Heart-rate, metabolic-rate and plasma lactate concentration during exercise
.
J. Exp. Biol.
179
,
31
-
46
.
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Haun
,
J. E.
and
Friedl
,
W. A.
(
1999
).
The diving physiology of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). I. Balancing the demands of exercise for energy conservation at depth
.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
2739
-
2748
.
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Fuiman
,
L. A.
,
Kendall
,
T.
,
Berry
,
P.
,
Richter
,
B.
,
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Thometz
,
N.
,
Shattock
,
M. J.
,
Farrell
,
E.
,
Stamper
,
A. M.
, et al. 
(
2015
).
Exercise at depth alters bradycardia and incidence of cardiac anomalies in deep-diving marine mammals
.
Nat. Commun.
6
,
6055
.
Wilson
,
R. P.
,
White
,
C. R.
,
Quintana
,
F.
,
Halsey
,
L. G.
,
Liebsch
,
N.
,
Martin
,
G. R.
and
Butler
,
P. J.
(
2006
).
Moving towards acceleration for estimates of activity-specific metabolic rate in free-living animals: the case of the cormorant
.
J. Anim. Ecol.
75
,
1081
-
1090
.
Wisniewska
,
D. M.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Teilmann
,
J.
,
Rojano-Doñate
,
L.
,
Shearer
,
J.
,
Sveegaard
,
S.
,
Miller
,
L. A.
,
Siebert
,
U.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2016
).
Ultra-high foraging rates of harbor porpoises make them vulnerable to anthropogenic disturbance
.
Curr. Biol.
26
,
1441
-
1446
.
Wright
,
A. K.
,
Ponganis
,
K. V.
,
McDonald
,
B. I.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2014
).
Heart rates of emperor penguins diving at sea: implications for oxygen store management
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
496
,
85
-
98
.
Zapol
,
W. M.
,
Liggins
,
G. C.
,
Schneider
,
R. C.
,
Qvist
,
J.
,
Snider
,
M. T.
,
Creasy
,
R. K.
and
Hochachka
,
P. W.
(
1979
).
Regional blood flow during simulated diving in the conscious Weddell seal
.
J. Appl. Physiol.
47
,
968
-
973
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information