Learning and memory provide the flexibility an organism requires to respond to changing social and ecological conditions. Juvenile Lymnaea have previously been shown to have a diminished capacity to form long-term memory (LTM) following operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behavior. Juvenile Lymnaea, however, can form LTM following classical conditioning of appetitive behaviors. Here, we demonstrate that laboratory-reared juvenile Lymnaea have the ability to detect the presence of a sympatric predator (i.e. crayfish) and respond to the predator by altering their aerial respiratory behavior. In addition to increasing their total breathing time, predator detection confers on juvenile Lymnaea an enhanced capability to form LTM following operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behavior. That is, these juveniles now have the ability to form long-lasting memory. These data support the hypothesis that biologically relevant levels of stress associated with predator detection induce behavioral phenotypic alterations (i.e. enhanced LTM formation) in juveniles, which may increase their fitness. These data also support the notion that learning and memory formation in conjunction with predator detection is a form of inducible defense.

In adult Lymnaea aerial respiratory behavior can be operantly conditioned by applying a tactile stimulus to the pneumostome area (the respiratory orifice) as the snail attempts to open it to breathe aerially. After repeated contingent pneumostome stimulation snails demonstrate learning and memory formation by opening their pneumostomes fewer times. This form of associative learning can then be consolidated into long-term memory (LTM) (Lukowiak et al., 1996; Lukowiak et al., 2003a; Lukowiak et al., 2003b; Lowe and Spencer, 2006; Khan and Spencer, 2009). Neural correlates of LTM have been found in RPeD1 (Spencer et al., 1999; Spencer et al., 2002; McComb et al., 2005b), one of the three central pattern generator neurons (Syed et al., 1990) that drive this behavior. Moreover, this neuron is a necessary site for the processes of memory consolidation, reconsolidation, extinction and forgetting (Scheibenstock et al., 2002; Sangha et al., 2003a; Sangha et al., 2003b; Sangha et al., 2003c; Sangha et al., 2003d; Sangha et al., 2005) However, the story in juvenile Lymnaea is different. In juveniles, RPeD1 spontaneous activity is significantly higher than in adults (McComb et al., 2003) and this may partially explain why juvenile Lymnaea learn significantly less well than adults not to perform aerial respiration following operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behavior and do not form LTM (McComb et al., 2005a). This leads us to ask: could predator detection in juvenile Lymnaea alter their ability to form LTM?

Predator detection elicits so-called inducible defenses which allow an organism to rapidly develop defensive morphological and behavioral phenotypes to ward off predation (Dodson, 1988; Larsson and Dodson, 1993). These defensive strategies include alteration of behaviors to enable an adaptive strategy best suited to the organism and the environmental demand. These cue-induced responses may allow the organism to reap the benefits of the defense only when advantageous to the organism without the cost associated with the full-time maintenance of the defensive strategy.

Our model system has been used to show how the stress associated with the detection of a naturally occurring predator alters cognitive behaviors in adult snails. Crayfish are a sympatric predator of Lymnaea in The Netherlands where the snails used in this study were originally collected in the 1950s. Our laboratory-bred snails demonstrate defensive behavioral strategies when crayfish scent is detected (Orr et al., 2007; Orr et al., 2009); as do freshly collected snails from other locations (Chivers and Smith, 1998; Covich et al., 1994; Orr et al., 2008; Rundle and Bronmark, 2001). Aerial respiratory behavior in Lymnaea, which is driven by a three-neuron network, the sufficiency and necessity of which has been experimentally verified (Syed et al., 1990; Syed et al., 1992), is also significantly altered when a snail detects a predator (Orr et al., 2007; Orr et al., 2009). As stated above, this respiratory behavior can be operantly conditioned to induce LTM (Lukowiak et al., 1998; Lukowiak et al., 2000); the duration of which is dependent, in part, on the training procedure used. Thus, in adult laboratory-reared and wild Netherlands snails, a single 30 min training session results in intermediate-term memory (ITM) that persists for up to 3 h; while two 30-min training sessions with a 1-h rest interval results in long-term memory (LTM) that persists for 24 h (Sangha et al., 2003a; Lukowiak et al., 2000; Lukowiak et al., 2003b; Parvez et al., 2005; Parvez et al., 2006a; Orr et al., 2008). Finally we report that when adult laboratory-bred Lymnaea are faced with the `smell' of a crayfish predator, memory formation is significantly enhanced and the correlates of this augmented LTM can be observed concurrently in both the whole animal behavior and in the electrophysiological characteristics of RPeD1 (Orr and Lukowiak, 2008).

A hypothesis we have developed (Orr et al., 2008; Orr et al., 2009) is that enhanced LTM formation as a result of predator detection is an anti-predator response. Thus, it is possible that predator detection in juvenile Lymnaea will enable these snails to form LTM. Here we demonstrate that when juvenile Lymnaea are faced with a biologically relevant stressor, the `smell' of a crayfish predator, they gain the ability to form LTM.

Animals

Lymnaea stagnalis L. were reared in our snail facility at the University of Calgary from a strain of Lymnaea originally obtained from Vrije Universeit in Amsterdam. The ancestors of these snails were collected from ditches in a polder located in the province of Utrecht in the early 1950s (Orr et al., 2008; Orr et al., 2009). Cohorts of 10–14 juvenile snails (McComb et al., 2005a) with a shell length of 12–17 mm (mean 14 mm), as measured from the apex of the spiral to the distal prominence of the apertural opening, were labeled and maintained within home aquaria at room temperature (≈20°C) until training.

Brief description of aerial respiratory behavior

Lymnaea are bimodal breathers obtaining oxygen through either cutaneous respiration (i.e. directly through their skin) or through aerial respiration via a rudimentary lung [i.e. gas exchange with the atmosphere; Lukowiak et al. (Lukowiak et al., 1996)]. To perform aerial respiration, the snail must surface and open its pneumostome (the respiratory orifice) while contracting and relaxing the appropriate respiratory muscles (Taylor and Lukowiak, 2000). Thus, training snails not to perform aerial respiration is not harmful. For a more detailed description of aerial respiratory behavior the reader is directed to two recent reviews (Lukowiak et al., 2003b; Lukowiak et al., 2008).

Exposure to crayfish effluent

Crayfish of the genus Cambrus were housed in a 110 l aquarium and maintained on a diet of snails and lettuce. We trained snails (see below) using 500 ml of water taken from the crayfish aquaria. We termed this water CE [crayfish effluent; see Orr et al. (Orr et al., 2007; Orr and Lukowiak, 2008)]. During the CE procedures, snails are not in the presence of crayfish, only water taken from aquaria containing crayfish.

Breathing observations

To determine if exposure to CE altered aerial respiratory behavior, snails were first placed in 500 ml of room-temperature hypoxic pond water (PO2<931 Pa) and then after a 24-h rest interval, placed in 500 ml of hypoxic (PO2<931 Pa) CE. The water is made hypoxic by bubbling N2 gas through the water for 20 min prior to introducing the snails. This is standard technique for measuring the aerial respiratory behavior and operant conditioning of this behavior in Lymnaea (Haney and Lukowiak, 2001; Lukowiak et al., 2003a; Lukowiak and Syed, 1999; Orr et al., 2007; Parvez et al., 2006a). The time and duration of the pneumostome openings were noted during each of the 0.5-h periods. From these measurements, the number of openings, total breathing time and average breathing time were calculated.

Operant conditioning

Snails were removed from home aquaria and placed into a 1-l beaker containing 500 ml of hypoxic pond water at an ambient temperature of approximately 20°C. The animals were given a 10-min acclimatization period prior to the 30-min training session. By subjecting snails to a hypoxic challenge, the animals increase their rate of aerial respiration (Lukowiak et al., 1996). The animals were operantly conditioned by applying a gentle tactile stimulus with a sharpened wooden applicator to their pneumostome as it begins to open (i.e. an attempted pneumostome opening). The stimulus is strong enough to cause the snails to close the pneumostome yet gentle enough that the snails do not perform the full body withdrawal response. This pneumostome closer response is a graded part of the whole-snail escape response (Inoue et al., 1996). Every time the snail opens its pneumostome and receives the stimulus during the training period, the time is recorded for future use in the yoked control experiments. Yoked controls (see below) were performed for all behavioral experiments. All behavioral experiments were done `blind' where the person performing the training paradigm was unaware of the status of the cohort being tested (i.e. in CE or pond water).

Two training paradigms were used: (1) the one-session training procedure and (2) the two-session training procedure [similar to that of Orr and Lukowiak (Orr and Lukowiak, 2008)]. The one-session procedure consisted of a single 0.5-h training session (TS) in hypoxia after which the snails were returned to their home eumoxic (i.e. normal O2 levels; PO2>9975 Pa) aquaria. The snails were then tested for memory (MT; i.e. a `savings-test') using a similar test to that of the training. The time of the memory test was at the indicated times after the TS. The two-session procedure consisted of two 0.5-h training sessions separated by a 1-h interval. The snails were placed into a eumoxic holding tank for the 1-h interval between TS1 and TS2 and after TS2. The memory test was performed at the indicated times after TS2.

Yoked control experiments

Snails that received yoked training were treated in an identical manner to that outlined in the Operant conditioning procedure section except that in the `training period', the stimuli were not contingent upon pneumostome opening. That is, the yoked control snails received the same number of stimuli in the exact same temporal pattern as the operantly conditioned snails but the stimuli were not contingent upon the opening of their pneumostome (Lukowiak et al., 2000; Lukowiak et al., 2003a; Lukowiak et al., 2008). The yoked snails did receive a contingent stimulus to the pneumostome during the savings test session (e.g. 24 h yoke).

Operational definition of learning and memory

As previously described (Lukowiak et al., 2000; Martens et al., 2007) for one-session training, memory is defined as a significant reduction in the number of pneumostome openings after training. That is, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the memory test session must be significantly less than TS1. In the case of two-session training, TS2 must be significantly lower than TS1 and the number of attempted openings in the memory test session must be significantly less than in TS1 but must also not be significantly greater than TS2.

Statistics

We analyzed water treatment effects on snail breathing behavior data with paired sample t-tests. All between-groups tests (i.e. between various TS1 or MT treatment) were analyzed using one-way ANOVA after homogeneity of variance tests were passed. All statistics were performed using SPSS, version 11.0.4 for Macintosh.

Predator detection and the respiratory behavior

We have demonstrated that adult Lymnaea both detect and respond to the scent of a crayfish predator (i.e. CE exposure) with multiple predator-evoked vigilance behaviors such as increasing their shadow elicited full-body withdrawal response, exploratory/search phase and their aerial respiratory behavior (Orr et al., 2007). Here we utilized the robust increase in aerial respiratory behavior during predator detection as a means to determine if juvenile snails can also detect the scent of this predator. We found that like adults, juvenile Lymnaea alter their aerial respiratory behavior in the presence of CE (Fig. 1). During a 0.5-hour observation period in each context (pond water vs CE), significant increases in both the total breathing time and the average breathing time per opening were observed in the snails exposed to CE (Fig. 1B,C). Interestingly, the total number of pneumostome openings was not different between each treatment. These results indicate that although juvenile snails do not open their respiratory orifice more often while exposed to CE, they do increase significantly their total breathing time.

Operant conditioning of juvenile snails in the presence of predator scent

Using the Lymnaea model system, we have utilized different training paradigms to evoke different forms and durations of memory. We have demonstrated that adult Lymnaea, when trained with the two-session training procedure (i.e. two 0.5-h sessions with a 1-h interval between sessions) in pond water form an LTM that persists for at least 24 h but not 48 h (Lukowiak et al., 2000; Parvez et al., 2006a; Parvez et al., 2006b; Lukowiak et al., 2007; Rosenegger et al., 2008). We recently reported that exposure to CE during the two-session operant conditioning procedure results in a dramatic (at least an eightfold) increase in memory duration in adult snails (Orr and Lukowiak, 2008). We have also demonstrated that juvenile Lymnaea when trained with the two-session training procedure in pond water fail to form LTM (McComb et al., 2005a). We therefore asked the question: does exposure to CE during the two-session operant conditioning procedure alter the ability of juvenile snails to form LTM? To answer this question, we first selected a naïve cohort of juvenile snails and subjected them to the two-session operant conditioning procedure in pond water and tested them for memory 24 h later. In pond water, learning was observed (i.e. the number of attempted openings in TS2 was significantly fewer than in TS1) but no memory was present 24 h later, confirming the earlier McComb et al. (McComb et al., 2005a) study (i.e. the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the memory test session was not significantly less than TS1; Fig. 2, top panel). A yoked control group also demonstrated no LTM as the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the yoked memory test was not significantly different from TS1 or for that matter from the 24-h memory test of snails that received operant conditioning training (Fig. 2). We next trained a new, naïve cohort of juvenile snails in CE and tested for memory 24 h later (Fig. 2, bottom panel). We found that LTM was present. That is, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the memory test was significantly lower than both TS1 and TS2 (Fig. 2, bottom panel). Yoked control snails `trained' in CE, by contrast, did not demonstrate memory at 24 h (Fig. 2, bottom panel).

Fig. 1.

Aerial respiratory behavior and predator detection. The mean (±s.e.m.) number of pneumostome openings (top panel); total breathing time (middle panel); and average breathing time per pneumostome opening (lower panel) of juvenile Lymnaea in pond water (PW) and crayfish effluent (CE). (Top) Number of pneumostome openings in PW=2.83±0.3, compared with CE=2.92±0.4 s.e.m. Treatments were not significantly different (P>0.05, N=37, paired t-test). (Middle) The total breathing time in PW was 72.41±8.18, and in CE, 163.86±22.05 (measured in seconds). The total breathing time in CE was significantly higher (**P<0.001, N=37) than in PW. (Bottom) The average breathing time in CE was significantly higher (56.22±6.22) than in PW (22±2.08, N=37, **P<0.001). Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 1.

Aerial respiratory behavior and predator detection. The mean (±s.e.m.) number of pneumostome openings (top panel); total breathing time (middle panel); and average breathing time per pneumostome opening (lower panel) of juvenile Lymnaea in pond water (PW) and crayfish effluent (CE). (Top) Number of pneumostome openings in PW=2.83±0.3, compared with CE=2.92±0.4 s.e.m. Treatments were not significantly different (P>0.05, N=37, paired t-test). (Middle) The total breathing time in PW was 72.41±8.18, and in CE, 163.86±22.05 (measured in seconds). The total breathing time in CE was significantly higher (**P<0.001, N=37) than in PW. (Bottom) The average breathing time in CE was significantly higher (56.22±6.22) than in PW (22±2.08, N=37, **P<0.001). Values are means ± s.e.m.

Having demonstrated that juvenile snails trained in CE now had the capability of forming LTM we next sought to determine if CE exposure enhanced the duration of memory (Fig. 3). To this end, we trained new cohorts of snails with the two-session training procedure in CE and tested memory at either 48 or 72 h. We also ran concurrent yoked groups for these time periods. We found that juvenile snails demonstrated LTM for up to 48 h but not 72 h when trained with the two-session procedure in CE (Fig. 3). As with the 24-hour yoked control snails, yoked control snails at 48 and 72 h did not demonstrate memory (Fig. 3).

In adult snails, we have found that the one-session 0.5-h training procedure in pond water results in intermediate-term memory (ITM) that persists for 3 but not 24 h. However, when the single training procedure is used in CE a 48 h memory results (Orr and Lukowiak, 2008). We, therefore, were interested to determine in juvenile snails the duration of memory resulting from a single 0.5-h session training procedure in both pond water and CE. A novel cohort was trained and tested in pond water 24 h later, and not surprisingly, memory was not demonstrated (Fig. 4, top). The yoked group also did not demonstrate a change in behavior from the naïve state (Fig. 4, top). By contrast, juveniles when trained similarly in CE demonstrated LTM at 24 but not 48 h (Fig. 4); whereas the 24 h yoke control group also did not demonstrate LTM (Fig. 4, bottom). These data demonstrate that in juvenile Lymnaea exposure to CE during operant conditioning training results in an enhanced ability to form LTM. Furthermore, just as in adult snails, the strength of the LTM formed in juvenile snails can be differentially and specifically induced using different training procedures. However, the LTM formed in juvenile snails exposed to CE is of a shorter duration than the enhanced duration of LTM seen in adults (Orr and Lukowiak, 2008).

Fig. 2.

Crayfish effluent (CE) enables juvenile Lymnaea to form long-term memory (LTM). (Top panel) In pond water (PW) the two-session training procedure resulted in learning but not LTM. That is, there was a significant reduction in the number of attempted pneumostome openings in training session 2 (TS2) compared with TS1 (TS1, 4.20±0.74; TS2 3.03±0.53; N=40, **P<0.01) but the 24-hour memory test was not significantly different from TS1 (24-h memory test, 4.12±0.49; N=40, P>0.05). Yoked controls (N=40) did not demonstrate memory, as the 24-h yoked memory test was not significantly different from TS1 (Y24h, 4.39±0.29; N=40, P>0.05, gray bar). (Bottom panel) In CE, the two-session training paradigm resulted in both learning and LTM. That is, TS2 (2.79±0.58) was significantly less (**P<0.01) than TS1 (4.00±0.78, N=40). Moreover, the number of attempted openings in the 24 h MT (2.02±0.41, N=25) was significantly less than TS1 (**P<0.01) whereas in the Y24h MT (N=25) the number of attempted openings (4.1±0.65) was not significantly different from TS1 (**P>0.05). Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 2.

Crayfish effluent (CE) enables juvenile Lymnaea to form long-term memory (LTM). (Top panel) In pond water (PW) the two-session training procedure resulted in learning but not LTM. That is, there was a significant reduction in the number of attempted pneumostome openings in training session 2 (TS2) compared with TS1 (TS1, 4.20±0.74; TS2 3.03±0.53; N=40, **P<0.01) but the 24-hour memory test was not significantly different from TS1 (24-h memory test, 4.12±0.49; N=40, P>0.05). Yoked controls (N=40) did not demonstrate memory, as the 24-h yoked memory test was not significantly different from TS1 (Y24h, 4.39±0.29; N=40, P>0.05, gray bar). (Bottom panel) In CE, the two-session training paradigm resulted in both learning and LTM. That is, TS2 (2.79±0.58) was significantly less (**P<0.01) than TS1 (4.00±0.78, N=40). Moreover, the number of attempted openings in the 24 h MT (2.02±0.41, N=25) was significantly less than TS1 (**P<0.01) whereas in the Y24h MT (N=25) the number of attempted openings (4.1±0.65) was not significantly different from TS1 (**P>0.05). Values are means ± s.e.m.

We demonstrated here that laboratory-reared juvenile Lymnaea have the ability to detect the presence of a crayfish predator; and that detection of the predator significantly alters their cognitive abilities. Juvenile Lymnaea do not have the capacity to form LTM following operant conditioning of aerial respiration, however, upon detection of a sympatric predator (e.g. crayfish) they gain the competence to form LTM.

The data presented here confirm the earlier findings (McComb et al., 2005a) that juvenile Lymnaea do not have the capability to form LTM following operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behavior. This should not be taken to mean that juvenile Lymnaea are incapable of learning or forming LTM. Juvenile Lymnaea, similar to other animals, have the capacity to learn and form LTM on tasks not involving the withholding of a behavior. Thus, for example, juvenile Lymnaea learn and form memory for appetitive food behaviors (Yamanaka et al., 2000). Juveniles of all species typically perseverate on tasks where they have to withhold a response, including habituation (Lukowiak, 1980), suppression of attack responses (Dickel et al., 1997) passive avoidance (Blozovski and Cudennec, 1980; Mattingly and Zolman, 1980), spatial discrimination (Bronstein and Spear, 1972) and the classical conditioning of Lymnaea's whole-body withdrawal response (Ono et al., 2002).

Fig. 3.

Training juveniles in crayfish effluent (CE) resulted in a long-term memory (LTM) that persisted for 48 h. A naive cohort of juvenile snails (N=93) was subjected to the two-trail training procedure and LTM was tested (MT sessions) once in randomly selected snails at 24, 48, and 72 h, respectively. Juvenile snails demonstrated LTM 24 and 48 h after TS2 but not 72 h later. That is, the snails demonstrated reduced pneumostome openings during TS2 (2.6±0.39, N=93, **P<0.01), 24-hour memory test (2.1±0.28, N=25, **P<0.01) and the 48-h memory test (2.8±0.38, N=38, **P<0.01) compared to the TS1 (4.29±0.47). However, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the 72 h MT (3.9±0.47, N=30, P>0.05) was not significantly less than TS1; thus, LTM was not present. As well, the 24-, 48- and 72-h yoked controls (N=25, 38, 30, respectively) were also not significantly different from the TS1 (P>0.05 in each comparison).

Fig. 3.

Training juveniles in crayfish effluent (CE) resulted in a long-term memory (LTM) that persisted for 48 h. A naive cohort of juvenile snails (N=93) was subjected to the two-trail training procedure and LTM was tested (MT sessions) once in randomly selected snails at 24, 48, and 72 h, respectively. Juvenile snails demonstrated LTM 24 and 48 h after TS2 but not 72 h later. That is, the snails demonstrated reduced pneumostome openings during TS2 (2.6±0.39, N=93, **P<0.01), 24-hour memory test (2.1±0.28, N=25, **P<0.01) and the 48-h memory test (2.8±0.38, N=38, **P<0.01) compared to the TS1 (4.29±0.47). However, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in the 72 h MT (3.9±0.47, N=30, P>0.05) was not significantly less than TS1; thus, LTM was not present. As well, the 24-, 48- and 72-h yoked controls (N=25, 38, 30, respectively) were also not significantly different from the TS1 (P>0.05 in each comparison).

However, here we found that if we trained juvenile Lymnaea in the presence of CE, they became competent in forming robust LTM. Thus, juvenile Lymnaea, as part of the response to predator detection, acquire the ability to form LTM. From this it follows that the neuronal network and the molecular changes in the neurons necessary for LTM formation following operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behavior in juveniles can be modified by this ecologically relevant stressor. For LTM consolidation to occur, both altered gene activity and new protein synthesis must occur in neurons such as RPeD1 that are sites for LTM formation following operant conditioning training (Sangha et al., 2003a; Scheibenstock et al., 2002). Future experiments will be performed to show that this indeed occurs. We will also attempt to determine the length of time that the juveniles must be exposed to CE for the necessary changes in neuronal circuitry to occur that allow for LTM formation.

The electrophysiological phenotype of RPeD1 in naïve juvenile snails recorded from in semi-intact preparations demonstrates that this neuron is more excitable than in adult snails (Klaassen et al., 1998; McComb et al., 2003). This difference in the electrophysiological properties is not seen when examining isolated ganglia from both juvenile and adult snails. These data suggest that the peripheral input from the pneumostome area and osphradial ganglion exert a suppressive regulatory control over the aerial respiratory central pattern generator which includes RPeD1 (Inoue et al., 2001; McComb et al., 2003). At this point we are not certain how the `smell' of the predator alters RPeD1 activity, if in fact it does in juvenile Lymnaea. These experiments are presently being performed. We also believe that RPeD1 activity in juveniles will be suppressed in CE and that the osphradium plays a significant role in both CE detection and signaling to CNS that results in the alteration of RPeD1 activity.

Fig. 4.

Long-term memory (LTM), crayfish effluent (CE) and the one-session operant conditioning procedure in juveniles. When we trained juvenile snails (N=30) using the less robust one-session training procedure in pond water (PW), they did not demonstrate memory after 24 h (top, white bars) or in the yoked control (N=30; top, gray bar). That is, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in memory training (MT; 3.9±0.15) was not significantly less than the number of attempted openings in TS1 (3.8±0.14; P>0.05). In addition the yoked control snails also did not demonstrate LTM (3.9±0.16). However when given one-session training in CE, the juveniles (N=108) demonstrated memory for at least 24 h. That is, the snails exhibited significantly reduced attempted pneumostome openings in the 24-h memory test (N=40, 1.96±0.14) compared with TS1 (4.04±0.19, **P<0.01). Juvenile snails did not demonstrate memory in the 48 h MT (N=68; 4.11±0.71) or during the 24 h yoked control (N=40, 4.09±0.15). Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 4.

Long-term memory (LTM), crayfish effluent (CE) and the one-session operant conditioning procedure in juveniles. When we trained juvenile snails (N=30) using the less robust one-session training procedure in pond water (PW), they did not demonstrate memory after 24 h (top, white bars) or in the yoked control (N=30; top, gray bar). That is, the number of attempted pneumostome openings in memory training (MT; 3.9±0.15) was not significantly less than the number of attempted openings in TS1 (3.8±0.14; P>0.05). In addition the yoked control snails also did not demonstrate LTM (3.9±0.16). However when given one-session training in CE, the juveniles (N=108) demonstrated memory for at least 24 h. That is, the snails exhibited significantly reduced attempted pneumostome openings in the 24-h memory test (N=40, 1.96±0.14) compared with TS1 (4.04±0.19, **P<0.01). Juvenile snails did not demonstrate memory in the 48 h MT (N=68; 4.11±0.71) or during the 24 h yoked control (N=40, 4.09±0.15). Values are means ± s.e.m.

We have found in adult snails that exposure to CE induces a transient reduction (<24 hours) in the spontaneous firing rate of RPeD1 and that the combination of operant conditioning in the presence of CE is required to prolong this reduction of neural activity for the duration of the memory (Orr et al., 2007; Orr and Lukowiak, 2008). Our current working hypothesis is that in juvenile snails, exposure to CE counteracts the increased excitability of RPeD1 and forces the network into a more `adult-like' configuration, thereby allowing operant conditioning to result in LTM formation. That is, the concurrent combination of suppressive input from peripheral centers during predator exposure and the operant conditioning procedure enhances the juvenile snail's ability to form robust LTM. Further investigations into this hypothesis are currently underway.

The term allelochemical is used to describe a chemical that conveys information that mediates an interaction between two organisms (Burks and Lodge, 2002). The use of kairomones (an allelochemical that conveys information between different species) has been described in many situations including alarm signaling (Chivers and Smith, 1998; Jacobsen and Stabell, 1999), planktonic interactions (Larsson and Dodson, 1993) and predator–prey interactions (Abrams, 1994; Jacobsen and Stabell, 2004; Kats and Dill, 1998; Dalesman et al., 2006). In vertebrate models, the utilization of kairomones to study the relationship between stress and memory have traditionally included the responses of mice and rats to the presence of cat or fox doors (Kim and Diamond, 2002; Maren, 2005; Takahashi et al., 2005). These investigations, despite their closer relevance to clinical (i.e. the human condition) situations, present the added complexity of dealing with complex vertebrate neuroanatomy where any noted changes in the physiological state of the animal due to predator detection can only be correlated with changes in behavior.

In aquatic environments chemical cues are important signals between and among species (Burks and Lodge, 2002). Predator scent has been shown to effect behaviors such as feeding and locomotion resulting in changes in growth and reproduction rates of gastropods in both fresh and marine systems (Burks and Lodge, 2002; Chivers and Smith, 1998; Covich et al., 1994; Palmer, 1990). Gastropods are also known to use chemical cues to assess and reduce the risk of predation (Covich et al., 1994; Jacobsen and Stabell, 2004) by increasing anti-predator behaviors such as crawling out of the water or increasing the use of a refuge during a predator presence. Numerous studies involving the response of various freshwater snails to the presence of crayfish predators have demonstrated the potency of the kairomone involved (Burks and Lodge, 2002; Chivers and Smith, 1998; Covich et al., 1994; Jacobsen and Stabell, 1999; Jacobsen and Stabell, 2004; Kats and Dill, 1998; Rigby and Jokela, 2000; Ryer et al., 2004), however, the precise chemical(s) has yet to be identified.

Juvenile Lymnaea were found to be capable of detecting the presence of a crayfish predator via a kairomone and responding with a functionally appropriate response. That is, the juvenile snails significantly increase their total aerial respiratory behavior in CE. This finding is consistent with the observation that adult snails also increase their respiratory behavior or even crawl out of the water when shell-crushing predators are detected (Alexander and Covich, 1991; Levri, 1998; Orr et al., 2007). We did, however, find a difference in the respiratory response between the juveniles and adults. We observed that the juvenile snails did not increase their total number of pneumostome openings during the breathing observations in CE as the adults do (Orr et al., 2007). Instead the juveniles increased their average duration of the pneumostome opening. The functional significance for this is as of yet unknown. A possible explanation for this finding, as pointed out by Vogel (Vogel, 1983) is that juvenile snails living in an aquatic environment possess a low Reynolds number and an increased relative viscosity; thus, they may have to use relatively more energy than an adult to break the surface tension in order to open the pneumostome. To overcome this energy constraint they open their pneumostome for a longer time rather than more often (Daniels et al., 1999). Nonetheless, our data provides support for the hypothesis that in juvenile Lymnaea, the sensory network has reached a stage of development whereby they can both detect and respond appropriately to a natural predator threat. Finally, we have to differentiate between our results regarding aerial breathing behavior in the absence of the presentation of a tactile stimulus to the pneumostome (i.e. the data shown in Fig. 1) with the data obtained when snails were operantly conditioned (e.g. Fig. 2). CE exposure causes a significant increase in aerial respiration; whereas training in CE results in a significant decrease in the number of attempted pneumostome openings (i.e. memory). Typically, snails attempt to open their pneumostome more often during operant conditioning training than they do during breathing observation sessions. The reason for this is that in hypoxic conditions where we are only observing breathing behavior (i.e. no tactile stimulus to the pneumostome area) the snails are able to satisfy their O2 requirements; whereas because they close their pneumostome when they receive a tactile stimulus during operant conditioning training they do not satisfy their O2 requirement, thus they attempt in the training session to open the pneumostome more often. However, if they learn not to open their pneumostome to avoid the tactile stimulus, and remember this in the memory test session there will be fewer attempts to open the pneumostome. Thus, in CE snails breathe aerially more often, but with operant conditioning training they learn and remember better, thus fewer attempted openings are recorded.

The new data presented here provide support for our hypothesis that enhanced memory formation as a result of predator detection is a form of an inducible defense that, at least in Lymnaea, is manifest at a very young age. It is unclear how predator detection alters the nervous system of juvenile Lymnaea so that they gain the ability to form LTM following our operant conditioning procedure. However, this model system presents a unique opportunity to study chemically mediated predator–prey interactions at the behavioral, neuronal and biochemical levels. Through this model system, the tools are at hand to fully investigate the neurobiological mechanisms of behavioral defense induction from the naturally occurring populations to the molecules involved in these defenses. In doing so we can gain a better understanding of the specific mechanisms involved in these processes and how they may relate to both natural and laboratory-reared populations.

The authors would like to thank Hyo-jung Orr, David Rosenegger, Kara Martens, Kashif Parvez and Kim Browning for all their help and comments. This research was supported by CIHR.

Abrams
P. A.
(
1994
).
Should prey overestimate the risk of predation
.
Am. Nat.
144
,
317
-
328
.
Alexander
J. E.
,
Covich
A. P.
(
1991
).
Predation risk and avoidance-behavior in 2 fresh-water snails
.
Biol. Bull.
180
,
387
-
393
.
Blozovski
D.
,
Cudennec
A.
(
1980
).
Passive avoidance learning in the young rat
.
Dev. Psychobiol.
13
,
513
-
518
.
Bronstein
P. M.
,
Spear
N. E.
(
1972
).
Acquisition of a spatial discrimination by rats as a function of age
.
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
78
,
208
-
212
.
Burks
R. L.
,
Lodge
D. M.
(
2002
).
Cued in, Advances and opportunities in freshwater chemical ecology
.
J. Chem. Ecol.
28
,
1901
-
1917
.
Chivers
D. P.
,
Smith
R. J. F.
(
1998
).
Chemical alarm signaling in aquatic predator-prey systems, A review and prospectus
.
Ecoscience
5
,
338
-
352
.
Covich
A. P.
,
Crowl
T. A.
,
Alexander
J. E.
,
Vaughn
C. C.
(
1994
).
Predator-avoidance responses in fresh-water decapod-gastropod interactions mediated by chemical stimuli
.
J. N. Am. Benthol. Soc.
13
,
283
-
290
.
Dalesman
S.
,
Rundle
S. D.
,
Coleman
R. A.
,
Cotton
P. A.
(
2006
).
Cue association and antipredator behaviour in a pulmonate snail, Lymnaea stagnalis
.
Anim. Behav.
71
,
789
-
797
.
Daniels
C. B.
,
Wood
P. G.
,
Loptako
O. V.
,
Codd
J. R.
,
Johnston
S. D.
,
Orgeig
S.
(
1999
).
Surfactant in the gas mantle of the snail Helix aspersa
.
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
72
,
691
-
698
.
Dickel
L.
,
Chichery
M. P.
,
Chichery
R.
(
1997
).
Postembryonic maturation of the vertical lobe complex and early development of predatory behavior in the cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis)
.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
67
,
150
-
160
.
Dodson
S.
(
1988
).
The ecological role of chemical stimuli for the zooplankton-predator-avoidance behavior in Daphnia
.
Limnol. Oceanog.
33
,
1431
-
1439
.
Haney
J.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2001
).
Context learning and the effect of context on memory retrieval in Lymnaea
.
Learn. Mem.
8
,
35
-
43
.
Inoue
T.
,
Takasaki
M.
,
Lukowiak
K.
,
Syed
N.
(
1996
).
Inhibition of the respiratory pattern-generating neurons by an identified whole-body withdrawal interneuron of Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
1887
-
1898
.
Inoue
T.
,
Haque
Z.
,
Lukowiak
K.
,
Syed
N. I.
(
2001
).
Hypoxia-induced respiratory patterned activity in Lymnaea originates at the periphery
.
J. Neurophysiol.
86
,
156
-
163
.
Jacobsen
H. P.
,
Stabell
O. B.
(
1999
).
Predator-induced alarm responses in the common periwinkle, Littorina littorea, dependence on season, light conditions, and chemical labeling of predators
.
Marine Biol.
134
,
551
-
557
.
Jacobsen
H. P.
,
Stabell
O. B.
(
2004
).
Antipredator behaviour mediated by chemical cues, the role of conspecific alarm signaling and predator labeling in the avoidance response of a marine gastropod
.
Oikos
104
,
43
-
50
.
Kats
L. B.
,
Dill
L. M.
(
1998
).
The scent of death, chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals
.
Ecoscience
5
,
569
-
569
.
Khan
A. M.
,
Spencer
G. E.
(
2009
).
Novel neural correlates of operant conditioning in normal and differentially reared Lymnaea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
922
-
933
.
Kim
J. J.
,
Diamond
D. M.
(
2002
).
The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories
.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
3
,
453
-
462
.
Klaassen
L. J.
,
Janse
C.
,
van der Roest
M.
(
1998
).
Multiple synaptic connections of a single neuron change differentially with age
.
Neurobiol. Aging
19
,
341
-
349
.
Larsson
P.
,
Dodson
S.
(
1993
).
Invited review-chemical communication in planktonic animals
.
Archiv. Fur. Hydrobiologie.
129
,
129
-
155
.
Levri
E. P.
(
1998
).
Perceived predation risk, parasitism, and the foraging behavior of a freshwater snail (Potamopyrgus antipodarum)
.
Can. J. Zool.
76
,
1878
-
1884
.
Lowe
M. R.
,
Spencer
G. E.
(
2006
).
Perturbation of the activity of a single identified neuron affects long-term memory formation in a molluscan semi-intact preparation
.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
711
-
721
.
Lukowiak
K.
(
1980
).
CNS control over gill reflex behaviors in Aplysia, satiation causes an increase in the suppressive control in older but not young animals
.
J. Neurobiol.
11
,
591
-
611
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Syed
N.
(
1999
).
Learning, memory and a respiratory central pattern generator
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
124A
,
265
-
274
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Ringseis
E.
,
Spencer
G.
,
Wildering
W.
,
Syed
N.
(
1996
).
Operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behaviour in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
683
-
691
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Cotter
R.
,
Westly
J.
,
Ringseis
E.
,
Spencer
G.
(
1998
).
Long-term memory of an operantly conditioned respiratory behaviour pattern in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
201
,
877
-
882
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Adatia
N.
,
Krygier
D.
,
Syed
N.
(
2000
).
Operant conditioning in Lymnaea, evidence for intermediate- and long-term memory
.
Learn Mem.
7
,
140
-
150
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Sangha
S.
,
McComb
C.
,
Varshney
N.
,
Rosenegger
D.
,
Sadamoto
H.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
(
2003a
).
Associative learning and memory in Lymnaea stagnalis, how well do they remember?
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
2097
-
2103
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Sangha
S.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
,
Parvez
K.
,
McComb
C.
,
Rosenegger
D.
,
Varshney
N.
,
Sadamoto
H.
(
2003b
).
A molluscan model system in the search for the engram
.
J. Physiol. Paris
97
,
69
-
76
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Fras
M.
,
Smyth
K.
,
Wong
C.
,
Hittel
K.
(
2007
).
Reconsolidation and memory infidelity in Lymnaea
.
Neurobiol. Learn Mem.
87
,
547
-
560
.
Lukowiak
K.
,
Martens
K.
,
Rosenegger
D.
,
Browning
K.
,
de Caigny
P.
,
Orr
M.
(
2008
).
The perception of stress alters adaptive behaviours in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
1747
-
1756
.
Maren
S.
(
2005
).
Building and burying fear memories in the brain
.
Neuroscientist
11
,
89
-
99
.
Martens
K.
,
de Caigny
P.
,
Parvez
K.
,
Amarell
M.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2007
).
Stressful stimuli modulate memory formation in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
Neurobiol. Learn Mem.
87
,
391
-
403
.
Mattingly
B. A.
,
Zolman
J. F.
(
1980
).
Ontogeny of passive avoidance learning in domestic chicks, punishment of key-peck and running responses
.
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
94
,
718
-
733
.
McComb
C.
,
Meems
R.
,
Syed
N.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2003
).
Electrophysiological differences in the CPG aerial respiratory behavior between juvenile and adult Lymnaea
.
J. Neurophysiol.
90
,
983
-
992
.
McComb
C.
,
Varshney
N.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2005a
).
Juvenile Lymnaea ventilate, learn and remember differently than do adult Lymnaea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
208
,
1459
-
1467
.
McComb
C.
,
Rosenegger
D.
,
Varshney
N.
,
Kwok
H.-Y.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2005b
).
Operant conditioning of an in vitro CNS-pneumostome preparation of Lymnaea
.
Neurobiol. Learning Mem.
84
,
9
-
24
.
Ono
M.
,
Kawai
R.
,
Horikoshi
T.
,
Yasuoka
T.
,
Sakakibara
M.
(
2002
).
Associative learning acquisition and retention depends on developmental stage in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
Neurobiol. Learn Mem.
78
,
53
-
64
.
Orr
M. V.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2008
).
Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence demonstrating that predator detection alters adaptive behaviors in the snail Lymnaea
.
J. Neurosci.
28
,
2726
-
2734
.
Orr
M. V.
,
El-Bekai
M.
,
Lui
M.
,
Watson
K.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2007
).
Predator detection in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
210
,
4150
-
4158
.
Orr
M.
,
Hittel
K.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2008
).
Comparing memory forming capabilities between laboratory-reared and wild Lymnaea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
2807
-
2816
.
Orr
M. V.
,
Hittel
K.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2009
).
`Different strokes for different folks': geographically isolated strains of Lymnaea stagnalis only respond to sympatric predators and have different memory forming capabilities
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
2237
-
2247
.
Palmer
A. R.
(
1990
).
Effect of crab effluent and scent of damaged conspecifics on feeding, growth, and shell morphology of the atlantic dogwhelk Nucella lapillus (L)
.
Hydrobiologia
193
,
155
-
182
.
Parvez
K.
,
Stewart
O.
,
Sangha
S.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2005
).
Boosting intermediate-term into long-term memory
.
J. Exp. Biol.
208
,
1525
-
1536
.
Parvez
K.
,
Moisseev
V.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2006a
).
A context specific single contingent-reinforcing stimulus boosts intermediate-term memory into long-term memory
.
Eur. J. Neurosci.
24
,
606
-
616
.
Parvez
K.
,
Rosenegger
D.
,
Martens
K.
,
Orr
M.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2006b
).
Canadian Association of Neurosciences Review, learning at a snail's pace
.
Can. J. Neurol. Sci.
33
,
347
-
356
.
Rigby
M. C.
,
Jokela
J.
(
2000
).
Predator avoidance and immune defense, costs and trade-offs in snails
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
267
,
171
-
176
.
Rosenegger
D.
,
Parvez
K.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2008
).
Enhancing memory formation by altering the balance of protein phosphorylation
.
Neurobiol. Learn Mem.
90
,
544
-
552
.
Rundle
S. D.
,
Bronmark
C.
(
2001
).
Inter- and intraspecific trait compensation of defense mechanisms in freshwater snails
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol.
268
,
1463
-
1468
.
Ryer
C. H.
,
Stoner
A. W.
,
Titgen
R. H.
(
2004
).
Behavioral mechanisms underlying the refuge value of benthic habitat structure for two flatfishes with differing anti-predator strategies
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
268
,
231
-
243
.
Sangha
S.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
,
McComb
C.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2003a
).
Intermediate and long-term memories of associative learning are differentially affected by transcription vs translation blockers in Lymnaea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
1605
-
1613
.
Sangha
S.
,
Morrow
R.
,
Smyth
K.
,
Cooke
R.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2003b
).
Cooling blocks ITM and LTM formation and preserves memory
.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
80
,
130
-
139
.
Sangha
S.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2003c
).
Reconsolidation of a long-term memory in Lymnaea requires new protein and RNA synthesis and the soma of RPeD1
.
J. Neurosci.
23
,
8034
-
8040
.
Sangha
S.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
,
Morrow
R.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2003d
).
Extinction requires new RNA and protein synthesis and the soma of the cell RPeD1 in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Neurosci.
23
,
9842
-
9851
.
Sangha
S.
,
Scheibenstock
A.
,
Martens
K.
,
Varshney
N.
,
Cooke
R.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2005
).
Impairing forgetting by preventing new learning and memory
.
Behav. Neurosci.
119
,
787
-
796
.
Scheibenstock
A.
,
Krygier
D.
,
Haque
Z.
,
Syed
N.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2002
).
The Soma of RPeD1 must be present for long-term memory formation of associative learning in Lymnaea
.
J. Neurophysiol.
88
,
1584
-
1591
.
Spencer
G. E.
,
Syed
N. I.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
1999
).
Neural changes after operant conditioning of the aerial respiratory behavior in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Neurosci.
19
,
1836
-
1843
.
Spencer
G. E.
,
Kazmi
M. H.
,
Syed
N. I.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2002
).
Changes in the activity of a CpG neuron after the reinforcement of an operantly conditioned behavior in Lymnaea
.
J. Neurophysiol.
88
,
1915
-
1923
.
Syed
N. I.
,
Bulloch
A. G.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
1990
).
In vitro reconstruction of the respiratory central pattern generator of the mollusk Lymnaea
.
Science
250
,
282
-
285
.
Syed
N. I.
,
Ridgway
R. L.
,
Lukowiak
K.
,
Bulloch
A. G.
(
1992
).
Transplantation and functional integration of an identified respiratory interneuron in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
Neuron
8
,
767
-
774
.
Takahashi
L. K.
,
Nakashima
B. R.
,
Hong
H.
,
Watanabe
K.
(
2005
).
The smell of danger: A behavioral and neural analysis of predator odor-induced fear
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
29
,
1157
-
1167
.
Taylor
B.
,
Lukowiak
K.
(
2000
).
The respiratory central pattern generator of Lymnaea, a model, measured and malleable
.
Respir. Physiol.
122
,
197
-
207
.
Vogel
S.
(
1983
).
Life in Moving Fluids, the Physical Biology of Flow
.
New Jersey, USA
:
Princeton University Press
.
Yamanaka
M.
,
Hatakeyama
D.
,
Sadamoto
H.
,
Kimura
T.
,
Ito
E.
(
2000
).
Development of key neurons for learning stimulates learning ability in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
Neurosci. Lett.
278
,
113
-
116
.