Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Daily energy expenditure in white storks is lower after fledging than in the nest
Andrea Flack, Paul J. Schaeffer, Jan R. E. Taylor, Inge Müller, Martin Wikelski, Wolfgang Fiedler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb219337 doi: 10.1242/jeb.219337 Published 28 January 2020
Andrea Flack
1Department of Migration, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
2Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany
3Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, D-78468 Konstanz, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Andrea Flack
  • For correspondence: aflack@ab.mpg.de
Paul J. Schaeffer
4Department of Biology, Miami University, 212 Pearson Hall, 700 E. High Street, Oxford, OH 45056, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jan R. E. Taylor
5Faculty of Biology, University of Białystok, ul. Ciołkowskiego 1J, 15-245 Białystok, Poland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Inge Müller
1Department of Migration, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
2Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany
3Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, D-78468 Konstanz, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Martin Wikelski
1Department of Migration, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
2Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany
3Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, D-78468 Konstanz, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Wolfgang Fiedler
1Department of Migration, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78315 Radolfzell, Germany
2Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany
3Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, D-78468 Konstanz, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Many juvenile birds turn into long-distance migrants within weeks of fledging. This transition involves upheavals in their energy management as major changes in growth and activity occur. Understanding such ontogenetic transitions in energy allocation has been difficult because collecting continuous data on energy costs in wild developing birds was previously largely impossible. Here, we continuously measured heart rate and fine-scale movements of 20 free-living juvenile white storks (Ciconia ciconia) using on-board bio-loggers to explore individual and environmental factors relating to daily mean heart rate. In addition, we explored which specific energy management strategy storks use during these crucial early life stages. We found that daily mean heart rate increased with overall movement activity, and increasing body temperature, but that it decreased with age. Further, we found that during the nestling period, when growth costs are high, activity costs are low, and post-fledging that activity costs are increased while maintenance costs are low, indicating a constraint on overall energy use in both phases. Our observations are consistent with the hypothesis that individuals invested more energy per unit time while still in the nest than after fledging despite the high costs of flight.

INTRODUCTION

Energy is a limited resource due to its finite availability in the environment and/or the restricted ability of individuals to acquire/spend it. Thus, animals are forced to allocate it carefully between their different behaviours and metabolic processes, thereby influencing fitness and survival (e.g. growth, migration, reproduction; Stearns, 1992). Because of the constraints on the amount of energy spent per day (i.e. daily energy expenditure, DEE), there is a potential trade-off between the two components of DEE: resting metabolic rate (RMR) and activity energy expenditure (Careau and Garland, 2012; Mathot and Dingemanse, 2015; Portugal et al., 2016; Halsey et al., 2019). RMR is defined as the lowest metabolic rate of an endotherm (juvenile or adult) at rest (Speakman et al., 2004); it corresponds to the costs of self-maintenance, including development, thermoregulation and immune functions. In recent years it has been shown that the minimum heart rate of an individual may serve as a proxy for RMR (Green, 2011). The difference between DEE, approximated by the average heart rate of an individual, and RMR represents energy spent specifically on energetically costly activities such as locomotion and/or foraging (Ricklefs et al., 1996; White et al., 2011). By examining the way in which the daily rate of energy expenditure, activity and RMR relate to each other, we may gain insights into an animal's energy management strategy and energy budget, and how these vary during different life stages. While many studies explore energy management strategies during a given life stage [e.g. breeding season (Hicks et al., 2018); moulting (Guillemmette et al., 2007)], we also know that energy management patterns can change during different life phases, or as a result of environmental factors (Halsey et al., 2019).

Within the first few months of life, young migrant birds experience a drastic transition in their physiology and behaviour, as they develop from nearly immobile nestlings to high-performance long-distance migrants. Examining the metabolic rate of developing wild migrants allows us to not only establish the relationships between the different components of energy expenditure at within-species and even within-individual levels but also examine the energy management strategies during the transition between these important life-history stages. Because of variation in activity, we expect that DEE (approximated by mean heart rate) varies during different developmental periods, and that changes in DEE give insight into the constraints acting on patterns of energy use (Mathot and Dingemanse, 2015). Given variation in DEE, an animal could follow three possible models of energy management patterns. (i) The independent model in which the amount of energy spent on activity is not related to the (constant) amount of energy spent on maintenance processes (Ricklefs et al., 1996; Careau and Garland, 2012; Hicks et al., 2018). Increases in RMR are predicted to have a positive relationship with DEE, as the former is a component of the latter, but with a slope smaller than unity as there is no relationship between RMR and activity (Mathot and Dingemanse, 2015; Portugal et al., 2016). (ii) The constrained or allocation model assumes that an animal maintains its overall energy expenditure within a narrow range (Deerenberg et al., 1998; Welcker et al., 2015), so that an increase in activity results in a decrease of maintenance energy expenditure or vice versa. In this case, we expect to observe no covariation between overall and maintenance energy expenditure (slope <1). (iii) The performance model is defined by an intrinsic link between daily and background energy expenditure, such that an increase in one is associated with an increase in the other (Careau et al., 2008), and is represented by positive covariation between overall and maintenance energy expenditure with a slope greater than 1 (as RMR is part of DEE).

Up to now, no continuous estimates of energy costs in wild birds that develop from fledgling to migrant have been collected, leaving a gap in our understanding of the energetic costs of this transition as well as of how birds may allocate their energy in this crucial life-history phase. Here, we examined long-term estimates of energy expenditure by looking at heart rate measurements as a qualitative proxy of energy expenditure rates (Green, 2011). We measured the heart rate and overall movements of wild white storks, Ciconia ciconia (Linnaeus 1758), to answer the following questions: which individual and environmental factors determine DEE during development?; how does DEE relate to RMR?; and what can this relationship tell us about different energy management strategies during the storks' early life stages?

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Ethics

Stork tagging in Poland was approved by Regional Directorate for Environmental Protection in Białystok (WPN.6401.50.2014.WL) and the Local Ethical Committee on Animal Experimentation in Białystok (73/2013). All methods conformed to the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research.

Dataset

Field work was conducted in the summers of 2014 and 2015 at the University of Białystok field station in Gugny, in Biebrza National Park (N 53°20′54.05″, E 22°35′33.85″). We tagged storks in three villages (Giełczyn: N 53°13′49″; E 22°29′04″; Brzeziny: N 53°14′21″; E 22°32′06″; and Laskowiec: N 53°13′52″; E 22°33′22″) located in the fork of the rivers Narew and Biebrza, close to the southern border of Biebrza National Park, northeast Poland. In order to record their movements, we equipped a total of 20 juvenile white storks (10 each year) with high-resolution, solar GSM-GPS-ACC loggers (e-Obs GmbH, Munich, Germany; Flack et al., 2016, 2018). All juveniles were taken from the nest to be fitted with the GPS logger (see Flack et al., 2016, 2018 for details) and a heart rate logger. We aimed to tag the birds approximately 1–2 weeks prior to fledging, but it was difficult to estimate precisely the age of the nestlings, so some birds took longer than 14 days to fledge. In addition, to measure heart rate, we implanted heart rate loggers into the abdominal cavity of the birds (9 g, 27×22×17 mm, e-Obs GmbH; see below). Every 20 min, the heart rate loggers recorded ECG (177.83 Hz) in bursts of 4.5 s (786 samples per burst) and abdominal temperature. GPS positions were recorded every 5 min. Tri-axial body acceleration was measured every 2 min (sampling details are given in Table S1). We recorded GPS locations, 3D body acceleration, body temperature (Tb) and an electrocardiogram (ECG) for 18 h a day (between 04:00 h and 22:00 h local time at the natal grounds). GPS transmitters (mass 54 g) were attached using a Teflon-nylon harness (mass ∼12 g). GPS had a positional accuracy of ±3.6 m (i.e. when stationary, 50% of fixes remained within a radius of 3.6 m within 24 h). For each measurement, the loggers recorded heart rate through two electrodes, temperature through an internal sensor, and a time stamp.

Average mass of the birds was 3010 g (range 2300–3600 g). Implantations were performed by a veterinarian (I.M.) in the field station to which the birds were transported by car. We administered a pre-anaesthetic analgesic (Butorphanol, 1.5 mg kg−1, intramuscular application) and Ringer's solution (at Tb, 20 ml kg−1, subcutaneous application) to prevent dehydration during anaesthesia. Sterilized loggers were implanted under isoflurane inhalation anaesthesia by inserting them into the abdominal cavity via a 4 cm long midline incision through the skin and body wall. The two flexible electrodes (diameter 3 mm, length 50 and 80 mm) were placed close to the heart. The strength of the signal was checked during implantation to achieve optimal position of the electrodes. The surgical incision was closed with a two-layer absorbable suture. Post-surgery, the birds received a non-steroidal anti-inflammatory analgesic (Meloxicam, 0.5 mg kg−1 body mass). Aseptic techniques were adhered to wherever possible. Birds were returned to the nest after recovering from anaesthesia and Tb was checked remotely during the following days. None of the birds showed a fever reaction.

All GPS loggers had a Global System for Mobile Communications (GSM) unit that sent two short text messages (SMS) per day (limited to areas of cellular coverage) each containing five GPS locations recorded at 1 h intervals, providing low-resolution positional data. These low-resolution data allowed us to find the animals in the field to download data of all sensors via a UHF radio link from a distance of approximately 300 m. We obtained simultaneous recording of position, accelerometer and ECG for the pre-migration period for 17 individuals. We also received data from all sensors during migration of 6 individuals, covering between 103.4 and 2670.2 km. Of the 20 tagged individuals, we excluded three birds completely from the analyses because of failure of the heart rate logger (1), or because the birds died shortly after their first flight (2). We analysed a total of 49,886 heart rate recordings, 162,396 GPS locations and 337,044 accelerometer recordings.

Data analyses

All statistical analysis was performed using R version (3.5.2, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). Raw acceleration data were converted from mV into m s−2 using the moveACC package (https://gitlab.com/anneks/moveACC). To calculate overall dynamic body acceleration (ODBA), the three signals were first individually smoothed using means of the entire burst. Next, for each axis, the smoothed data were subtracted from the corresponding unsmoothed data; the sum of all three axes provided ODBA (Wilson et al., 2006). ECG data were post-processed to obtain heart rate values for each recorded ECG burst (fH, beats min−1) using a custom-written detection algorithm (eOBS GmbH), which we manually verified. We determined daily mean fH for each 24 h period and individual as well as daily minimum fH, which corresponds to the lowest burst value of each day. In addition, we determined activity fH, defined as the difference between daily mean and daily minimum fH (Ricklefs et al., 1996; Careau et al., 2008; Portugal et al., 2016). We discarded the first 72 h after device implantation because of potential interference from the surgical procedure. We also discarded the day of death (determined using criteria from Cheng et al., 2019). We examined the pre- and post-fledging phases separately with fledging being defined as the first of three consecutive days during which the maximum distance to the nest was larger than 500 m. Because some individuals took longer to fledge (they might have been younger during tagging), we cut the pre-fledging phase to the last 12 days prior to fledging to examine the same number of days for all individuals. The post-fledging phase ended with the first day of migration (i.e. first day with a latitudinal difference of −0.38°). We collected migration data for only a few individuals and days: a total of 34 migration days for 5 individuals.

We ran general linear mixed models with individual animal ID as a random factor to determine the environmental and individual factors that influenced daily mean fH (DEE). Models were also implemented with a temporal autocorrelation structure (corAR1). For the final model, we selected only those factors that were retained in the four best GLMMs using the Akaike information criterion for small sample sizes (AICc) within the MuMIn library. Individual features included in the models were daily mean Tb and mean movement activity (ODBA). In the post-fledging model, we also included distance travelled (beeline distance between the first and last point of each day). We also included days until (or after) fledging to correct for variation in fH independent of individual and environmental conditions. We used the environmental-data automated track annotation (Env-DATA) system (Dodge et al., 2013) to annotate the tracking data with ambient atmospheric observations. Environmental conditions included mean ground temperature, mean precipitation, mean thermal uplift, mean wind support and mean cross-wind. Thermal uplift velocity estimates provided by Env-DATA were calculated using estimates of temperature, relative humidity, surface pressure, boundary layer height, and instantaneous moisture and surface heat fluxes using ECMWF data (Bohrer et al., 2011).

We assessed the use of alternative energy management patterns by examining the slope and 95% confidence intervals of regression between daily mean fH and daily minimum fH, and between minimum fH and activity fH. These regressions were generated from a single mixed model, including individual identity as a random effect to account for the repeated values representing each individual.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

First, we explored how individuals changed their movement activities during development. To do so, we calculated their daily mean ODBA to quantify the level of movement. ODBA was lowest when the birds had not yet fledged, ranging from 0.29 to 0.89 (Fig. 1B). It increased during the post-fledging phase (mean±s.d. daily ODBA 0.95±0.41), and reached a maximum during migration (daily ODBA 1.57±0.34; Fig. 1). In addition, daily mean ODBA was strongly positively correlated (GLMM, conditional R2=0.77, P<0.001) with cumulative flight distance (Fig. S1), indicating that it is a reliable estimate of flight behaviour.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Movement activity during development. (A) Daily overall dynamic body acceleration (ODBA) plotted against days relative to fledging (negative numbers correspond to the pre-fledging period). Lifetime stages are indicated: 0, pre-fledging; 1, post-fledging; and 2, migration phase. (B) Histogram of daily ODBA during the three different phases.

Next, we examined the costs of flight development by looking at daily mean fH (our measure of DEE) and its influencing factors. We found that daily mean fH decreased with time (Fig. 2A). In addition, it was positively influenced by daily mean ODBA (Fig. 2B), internal Tb (Fig. 2C) and daily travel distance (Fig. 2D; GLMM, marginal R2=0.38; Table S2). Thus, although the movement activity of the juveniles increased as their development progressed (Fig. 1A), daily mean fH was highest during their time in the nest (Fig. 2A).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Partial residual plots for the model examining the relationships between daily mean heart rate (fH) and its predictors. See Table S2. The plots include the partial fits (red solid lines) and their 95% confidence intervals (shaded grey areas). Partial residuals are plotted for (A) days relative to fledging (negative numbers correspond to the pre-fledging period); (B) daily mean ODBA, (C) daily mean body temperature (Tb); and (D) cumulative daily distance (m; log-transformed). Each fit is adjusted by the other covariates.

Finally, because we clearly observed a change in daily activity levels and energy expenditure throughout juvenile development, we tested whether this decrease in DEE is also linked to changes in the energy management pattern. To do so, we examined the relationship between daily mean fH and daily minimum fH (a measure of maintenance energy) for the pre- and post-fledging phase (including migratory movements) separately. In both phases, before and after fledging, we observed a strong positive relationship between mean daily fH and daily minimum fH. The across-individual slopes between these values were lower than 1 during both life stages (pre-fledging: 0.64; post-fledging/migration: 0.67; Fig. 3, Table S3), indicating that there is a partial compensation of high maintenance or high activity energy expenditure. This means that high maintenance costs (especially during the early development of the pre-fledging phase) are not equally connected to high levels of overall energy expenditure but are accompanied by lower activity levels [slope of daily fH and activity energy (difference between mean and minimum daily fH): −0.36, conditional R2=0.24]. Conversely, an increase of movement during the post-fledging/migration phase increased activity energy expenditure and coincided with a decrease in maintenance processes (GLMM, slope: −0.30; conditional R2=0.35).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Relationship between daily mean and minimum fH at different life stages. (A) Pre-fledging and (B) post-fledging/migration. Daily mean fH is a proxy for daily energy expenditure; daily minimum fH is a proxy for daily maintenance energy expenditure. The individual slopes are clearly >0 and <1 (see Table S3). Open circles represent all observations (one data point per individual per day). Thick black lines correspond to the across-individual slopes. The thin black line represents the line of unity.

Our study showed that in juvenile white storks, fH-based proxies for RMR and DEE are positively related to each other with across-individual slope >0 and <1. This means that high maintenance or activity energy will be partially compensated in order to constrain the overall level of DEE. High values of maintenance energy during the pre-fledging phase may be connected to prevailing growth processes. Although the beak and feathers of white storks continue to grow even after they have fledged, individuals generally reach their maximum mass before their first flight, ∼45 days after hatching (Tsachalidis et al., 2005). Thus, the decrease in maintenance energy expenditure during the pre-fledging phase may be connected to a reduction of overall body growth and development.

At the end of the nesting period, juveniles increase their movement activity as they leave their nests to fly and to forage on their own. Although activity fH increased together with movement activity (ODBA), we still observed a general reduction of DEE over the post-fledging period. This apparent disconnect between increased energetic costs during flight and the overall decrease in DEE with maturation suggests several possibilities. A reduction of DEE may indicate that the transition to independent living includes learning to fly more efficiently and/or to forage more effectively. This development may be part of processes preparing the juveniles for migration. It has been shown in the past that many migrants can undertake these incredible, long journeys without any apparent change in behaviour prior to their departure (Portugal et al., 2011; Hawkes et al., 2017). Large birds in particular have evolved behaviours such as soaring, gliding and thermalling to reduce flight costs (Duriez et al., 2014; Flack et al., 2016; Sherub et al., 2016), requiring them to flap very little and not to increase their muscle protein content prior to migration (Pennycuick, 1975).

Our intra-specific study of a developing bird also shows that DEE can vary during different developmental or life stages. During the early pre-fledging phase, juveniles had a larger DEE although they showed little movement activity. During this time, when food availability may be higher as a result of parental provisioning, nestling storks apparently spent more energy (higher minimum fH, presumably higher RMR) on growth to increase their muscle and lipid stores. Once individuals fledge, they experience not only an increase in activity levels but also lower, unknown feeding rates because of immature foraging skills. It may be important to experience lower maintenance costs during this phase to compensate for the high costs of activity. We must also consider that there are additional factors, including maturation of cardiovascular functions or varying responses to elevated Tb, which may all affect these outcomes. However, as our models suggest a strong effect of the overall maturation process, our proxy for energy use appears robust.

Because of high mortality in juvenile storks (Cheng et al., 2019), we did not succeed in recording the entire annual cycle of these birds. But our study nevertheless reveals that factors determining energy management strategies can vary throughout early ontogeny depending on different constraining factors. Because important stages of an animal's life cycle, like periods of high or low energetic costs (or food availability), may drive changes in energy management patterns, we need to further explore empirically and experimentally energetic constraints in individual free-living animals.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dina Dechmann for establishing this programme funded by the Max Planck-Poland Biodiversity Initiative. We thank e-Obs GmbH for their help with logger programming, and Matthias Berger for help with the analysis of the heart rate data.

FOOTNOTES

  • Competing interests

    The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

  • Author contributions

    Conceptualization: A.F., W.F.; Methodology: I.M.; Formal analysis: A.F.; Data curation: P.J.S.; Writing - original draft: A.F.; Writing - review & editing: A.F., P.J.S.; Project administration: J.R.E.T., W.F.; Funding acquisition: J.R.E.T., M.W., W.F.

  • Funding

    We acknowledge funding from the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology and the Max Planck-Poland Biodiversity Initiative. A.F. was supported by Deutsches Zentrum für Luft- und Raumfahrt and Christiane Nüsslein-Volhard Stiftung.

  • Supplementary information

    Supplementary information available online at http://jeb.biologists.org/lookup/doi/10.1242/jeb.219337.supplemental

  • Received November 26, 2019.
  • Accepted December 17, 2019.
  • © 2020. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd
http://www.biologists.com/user-licence-1-1/

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bohrer, G.,
    2. Brandes, D.,
    3. Mandel, J. T.,
    4. Bildstein, K. L.,
    5. Miller, T. A.,
    6. Lanzone, M.,
    7. Katzner, T.,
    8. Maisonneuve, C. and
    9. Tremblay, J. A.
    (2011). Estimating updraft velocity components over large spatial scales: contrasting migration strategies of golden eagles and turkey vultures. Ecol. Lett. 15, 96-103. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01713.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Careau, V.,
    2. Thomas, D.,
    3. Humphries, M. M. and
    4. Réale, D.
    (2008). Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos 117, 641-653. doi:10.1111/j.0030-1299.2008.16513.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  3. ↵
    1. Careau, V. and
    2. Garland, T.
    (2012). Performance, personality, and energetics: correlation, causation, and mechanism. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 85, 543-571. doi:10.1086/666970
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. ↵
    1. Cheng, Y.,
    2. Fiedler, W.,
    3. Wikelski, M. and
    4. Flack, A.
    (2019). “Closer-to-home” strategy benefits juvenile survival in a long-distance migratory bird. Ecol. Evol. 9, 8945-8952. doi:10.1002/ece3.5395
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    1. Deerenberg, C.,
    2. Overkamp, G. J. F.,
    3. Visser, G. H. and
    4. Daan, S.
    (1998). Compensation in resting metabolism for experimentally increased activity. J. Comp. Physiol. B 168, 507-512. doi:10.1007/s003600050171
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    1. Dodge, S.,
    2. Bohrer, G.,
    3. Weinzierl, R.,
    4. Davidson, S. C.,
    5. Kays, R.,
    6. Douglas, D.,
    7. Cruz, S.,
    8. Han, J.,
    9. Brandes, D. and
    10. Wikelski, M.
    (2013). The environmental-data automated track annotation (Env-DATA) system: linking animal tracks with environmental data. Mov. Ecol. 1, 3. doi:10.1186/2051-3933-1-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Duriez, O.,
    2. Kato, A.,
    3. Tromp, C.,
    4. Dell'omo, G.,
    5. Vyssotski, A. L.,
    6. Sarrazin, F. and
    7. Ropert-Coudert, Y.
    (2014). How cheap is soaring flight in raptors? a preliminary investigation in freely-flying vultures. PLoS ONE 9, e84887. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0084887
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Flack, A.,
    2. Fiedler, W.,
    3. Blas, J.,
    4. Pokrovsky, I.,
    5. Kaatz, M.,
    6. Mitropolsky, M.,
    7. Aghababyan, K.,
    8. Fakriadis, I.,
    9. Makrigianni, E.,
    10. Jerzak, L. et al.
    (2016). Costs of migratory decisions: a comparison across eight white stork populations. Sci. Adv. 2, e1500931. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1500931
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Flack, A.,
    2. Nagy, M.,
    3. Fiedler, W.,
    4. Couzin, I. D.
    5. Wikelski, M.
    (2018). From local collective behavior to global migratory patterns in white storks. Science 360, 911-914. doi:10.1126/science.aap7781
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Green, J. A.
    (2011). The heart rate method for estimating metabolic rate: review and recommendations. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 158, 287-304. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.09.011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
    1. Guillemette, M.,
    2. Pelletier, D.,
    3. Grandbois, J.-M. and
    4. Butler, P. J.
    (2007). Flightlessness and the energetic cost of wing molt in a large sea duck. Ecology 88, 2936-2945. doi:10.1890/06-1751.1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    1. Halsey, L. G.,
    2. Green, J. A.,
    3. Twiss, S. D.,
    4. Arnold, W.,
    5. Burthe, S. J.,
    6. Butler, P. J.,
    7. Cooke, S. J.,
    8. Grémillet, D.,
    9. Ruf, T.,
    10. Hicks, O. et al.
    (2019). Flexibility, variability and constraint in energy management patterns across vertebrate taxa revealed by long-term heart rate measurements. Funct. Ecol. 33, 260-272. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.13264
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    1. Hawkes, L. A.,
    2. Batbayar, N.,
    3. Butler, P. J.,
    4. Chua, B.,
    5. Frappell, P. B.,
    6. Meir, J. U.,
    7. Milsom, W. K.,
    8. Natsagdorj, T.,
    9. Parr, N.,
    10. Scott, G. R. et al.
    (2017). Do bar-headed geese train for high altitude flights? Integr. Comp. Biol. 57, 240-251. doi:10.1093/icb/icx068
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Hicks, O.,
    2. Burthe, S. J.,
    3. Daunt, F.,
    4. Newell, M.,
    5. Chastel, O.,
    6. Parenteau, C. and
    7. Green, J. A.
    (2018). The role of parasitism in the energy management of a free-ranging bird. J. Exp. Biol. 221, jeb190066. doi:10.1242/jeb.190066
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Mathot, K. J. and
    2. Dingemanse, N. J.
    (2015). Energetics and behavior: unrequited needs and new directions. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 199-206. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2015.01.010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Pennycuick, C. J.
    (1975). Mechanics of flight. Avian Biol. 5, 1-73. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-249405-5.50009-4
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Portugal, S. J.,
    2. Green, J. A.,
    3. White, C. R.,
    4. Guillemette, M. and
    5. Butler, P. J.
    (2011). Wild geese do not increase flight behaviour prior to migration. Biol. Lett. 8, rsbl20110975. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0975
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Portugal, S. J.,
    2. Green, J. A.,
    3. Halsey, L. G.,
    4. Arnold, W.,
    5. Careau, V.,
    6. Dann, P.,
    7. Frappell, P. B.,
    8. Grémillet, D.,
    9. Handrich, Y.,
    10. Martin, G. R. et al.
    (2016). Associations between resting, activity, and daily metabolic rate in free-living endotherms: no universal rule in birds and mammals. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 89, 251-261. doi:10.1086/686322
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Ricklefs, R. E.,
    2. Konarzewski, M. and
    3. Daan, S.
    (1996). The relationship between basal metabolic rate and daily energy expenditure in birds and mammals. Am. Nat. 147, 1047-1071. doi:10.1086/285892
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Sherub, S.,
    2. Bohrer, G.,
    3. Wikelski, M. and
    4. Weinzierl, R.
    (2016). Behavioural adaptations to flight into thin air. Biol. Lett. 12, 20160432. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2016.0432
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Speakman, J. R.,
    2. Król, E. and
    3. Johnson, M. S.
    (2004). The functional significance of individual variation in basal metabolic rate. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 77, 900-915. doi:10.1086/427059
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  21. ↵
    1. Stearns, S. C.
    (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.
  22. ↵
    1. Tsachalidis, E. P.,
    2. Liordos, V. and
    3. Goutner, V.
    (2005). Growth of white stork Ciconia ciconia nestlings. Ardea 93, 133-137.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    1. Welcker, J.,
    2. Speakman, J. R.,
    3. Elliott, K. H.,
    4. Hatch, S. A. and
    5. Kitaysky, A. S.
    (2015). Resting and daily energy expenditures during reproduction are adjusted in opposite directions in free-living birds. Funct. Ecol. 29, 250-258. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12321
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    1. White, C. R.,
    2. Grémillet, D.,
    3. Green, J. A.,
    4. Martin, G. R. and
    5. Butler, P. J.
    (2011). Metabolic rate throughout the annual cycle reveals the demands of an Arctic existence in great cormorants. Ecology 92, 475-486. doi:10.1890/09-1951.1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Wilson, R. P.,
    2. White, C. R.,
    3. Quintana, F.,
    4. Halsey, L. G.,
    5. Liebsch, N.,
    6. Martin, G. R. and
    7. Butler, P. J.
    (2006). Moving towards acceleration for estimates of activity-specific metabolic rate in free-living animals: the case of the cormorant. J. Anim. Ecol. 75, 1081-1090. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01127.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • Energy management model
  • Daily energy expenditure
  • Heart rate
  • Ciconia ciconia
  • Ontogeny

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Daily energy expenditure in white storks is lower after fledging than in the nest
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Daily energy expenditure in white storks is lower after fledging than in the nest
Andrea Flack, Paul J. Schaeffer, Jan R. E. Taylor, Inge Müller, Martin Wikelski, Wolfgang Fiedler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb219337 doi: 10.1242/jeb.219337 Published 28 January 2020
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Daily energy expenditure in white storks is lower after fledging than in the nest
Andrea Flack, Paul J. Schaeffer, Jan R. E. Taylor, Inge Müller, Martin Wikelski, Wolfgang Fiedler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb219337 doi: 10.1242/jeb.219337 Published 28 January 2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgements
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Strain-specific differences in muscle Ca2+ transport and mitochondrial electron transport chain proteins between FVB/N and C57BL/6J mice
  • Latencies of mechanically-stimulated escape responses in the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi
  • Rapid toxin sequestration modifies poison frog physiology
Show more SHORT COMMUNICATION

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


Critical temperature window sends migratory black-headed buntings on their travels

The spring rise in temperature at black-headed bunting overwintering sites is essential for triggering the physical changes that they undergo before embarking on their spring migration – read more.


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992