Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
Research Article
The visual spectral sensitivity of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta
Felipe Tapia, Jesús Olivares, Oliver Schmachtenberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb217133 doi: 10.1242/jeb.217133 Published 22 January 2020
Felipe Tapia
1Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaíso (CINV), Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Valparaíso, Valparaíso 2381850, Chile
2PhD Program in Neuroscience, Universidad de Valparaíso, Valparaíso 2360102, Chile
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Felipe Tapia
Jesús Olivares
1Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaíso (CINV), Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Valparaíso, Valparaíso 2381850, Chile
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Oliver Schmachtenberg
1Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaíso (CINV), Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Valparaíso, Valparaíso 2381850, Chile
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Oliver Schmachtenberg
  • For correspondence: oliver.schmachtenberg@uv.cl
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Spiders are a large group of arthropods and nearly omnipresent in warm and temperate climates. They rely on tactile and visual information to hunt and breed, but compared with their mechanical senses, little is known about their visual systems. In this study, we analyzed the visual spectral sensitivity of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta, a synanthropic species posing a significant threat to humans, using electroretinogram recordings of its three eye types and open field tests with localized chromatic illumination for behavioral analysis. The electroretinogram displayed two sensitivity peaks in the ultraviolet and green ranges, and no differences were observed between the three eye types and between male and female specimens. Selective chromatic adaptation reduced overall light sensitivity, but did not support the expression of more than one type of rhodopsin in photoreceptors. The open field tests revealed a preference for corners over side areas, and an increased exploration of open field areas illuminated by shorter wavelength (violet to green) light compared with non-illuminated areas, while no behavioral responses to red and near-infrared light were observed. These data suggest that L. laeta has monochromatic vision without spectral specializations in its three secondary eye pairs.

INTRODUCTION

There are over 48 thousand spider species on Earth (World Spider Catalog 20.5, https://wsc.nmbe.ch/, accessed on 9 January 2020), but comparatively little is known regarding their visual capabilities, although these aroused an early scientific interest. In the nineteenth century, it was shown that some spiders have the ability to distinguish colors, besides being able to recognize objects such as their egg cocoons and their conspecifics at close range (George and Peckham, 1887). Unlike the compound eyes of insects, spider eyes are simple lens eyes (Land and Nilsson, 2012). Most spider species have eight eyes arranged in two rows in the anterior region of their prosoma above the chelicerae, with those nearest to the clypeus usually classified as anterior lateral (AL) and anterior median (AM) eyes, while the more caudal eyes are termed posterior lateral (PL) and posterior median (PM) eyes. However, there are spider species that have fewer eyes. For instance, species of the families Dysderidae, Scytodidae, Pholcidae and Sicariidae have six eyes, while other species have only four eyes, such as representatives of the genus Tetrablemma (family Theridiidae), or two eyes, such as spiders of the family Caponiidae. Some cavern-dwelling species lack eyes completely (Foelix, 2011). The eyes of spiders can be classified as principal and secondary depending on their embryological origin, which has implications for their morphology and function. The AM eyes, generally referred to as principal eyes (Barth, 2002b; Yamashita, 1985), express photopigments in microvillous photoreceptor segments near the lens, unlike the secondary eyes, whose photoreceptors point in the opposite direction, with cell bodies closer to the lens and the photopigment-containing segment forming the first portion of the axon (Blest, 1985). Principal eyes are relevant for visual acuity, forming low-resolution images, while secondary eyes are thought to be related to navigation and may serve to detect celestial cues such as the sun or other stars at night (Land and Nilsson, 2012).

Certain spiders have very well-developed visual systems, such as species of the families Salticidae, Lycosidae and Thomisidae of mostly diurnal habits (Defrize et al., 2011; Nuboer, 1986). Examples of spiders with complex visual systems include the lycosids, which have the ability to detect polarized light, mainly used for spatial orientation (Dacke et al., 2001). Both principal and secondary eye types have a spectral sensitivity ranging from 380 to 580 nm, with photoreceptors that absorb maximally between 505 and 510 nm, and a second absorption peak in the AM eyes at 380 nm (DeVoe et al., 1969). Furthermore, thomisids, voracious hunters that ambush their prey, have the ability to choose the plants they use for hiding based not only on their shape but also on their spectral reflectance patterns, and can also change their body color to adapt to the environment. Thomisid spiders of the species Misumena vatia were reported to have two classes of photoreceptor cells, one with a sensitivity peak at 340 nm and another with maximum sensitivity around 520 nm (Defrize et al., 2011).

In contrast, salticids have up to four classes of photoreceptors in their principal eyes, based on their cellular spectral sensitivities, and are thought to possess color vision (Foelix, 2011; Nuboer, 1986). They are also able to judge distance precisely based on visual cues, ostensibly through an elaborate combination of chromatic aberration and image defocus, detected by four tiered photoreceptor layers in the eye (Nagata et al., 2012). The vision of some salticid species is necessary and sufficient to induce courtship behavior (Cross et al., 2008). Another exceptional case is the jumping spider Evarcha culicivora, which is able to detect whether a mosquito has recently consumed human blood, based on both olfactory and visual cues (Jackson et al., 2005; Nentwig, 2013). More recently, processing of visual information has been demonstrated in the visual brain centers of Phidippus audax (Menda et al., 2014). Among the species with nocturnal habits, probably the most studied is the wandering spider Cupiennius salei, a Central American species of the family Ctenidae, which has served as a model in many studies of spider biology since the 1960s (Barth, 2002a).

As mentioned above, spiders of the genus Loxosceles (Sicariidae) have six eyes arranged in three dyads (Fig. 1). The AM eyes are absent and the PM eyes have migrated to a more anterior position, remaining in a recurved disposition in relation to the lateral dyads (Gertsch and Ennik, 1983). These animals occupy a wide variety of habitats, both domestic and natural. As a species of synanthropic habits, they are usually found in fissures, spaces between furniture, and behind photographs and paintings (Gonçalves-de-Andrade et al., 2007), while in natural environments, several species of the genus live in cracks of trees, rock walls and caves (Canals et al., 2015a; Elliott et al., 2017; Tacaure-Ríos, 2011). Being a group of highly venomous species common in Latin America, with closely related albeit slightly less poisonous species inhabiting the southern USA, the Mediterranean and other temperate parts of the world, it seems relevant to better understand what sensory stimuli attract or repel species of the genus Loxosceles, and by what criteria they choose their habitat. However, to the best of our knowledge, there are no data available regarding the visual capabilities of any of the species of the genus Loxosceles, and only a handful of reports have shed light on its biology (Calbiague et al., 2017; Canals and Solís, 2014; Canals et al., 2015a,b,c).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

The eyes of Loxosceles laeta. (A) Male specimen of L. laeta. (B) Overview of the position of the three eye pairs of L. laeta. PM, posterior median; AL, anterior lateral; PL, posterior lateral. Note the missing AM eyes. (C) Toluidine Blue-stained semi-thin cross-section of L. laeta PM eyes. (D) Detail of a PM eye, showing the primitive-type secondary eye structure with photoreceptor somata close to the lens and rhabdoms towards the tapetum. L, lens; Ph, photoreceptors; Rh, rhabdoms; T, tapetum; P, pigment layer. Scale bars: A, 1 cm; B, 1 mm; C and D, 100 µm.

The Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta is considered the most dangerous species of this genus (Binford and Wells, 2003), as a result of its mostly synanthropic habits and highly necrotoxic venom, which may cause serious disfiguring tissue damage and potentially lethal systemic reactions in a minor percentage of the bites (Chaves-Moreira et al., 2017). However, despite its public health relevance, next to nothing is known about the sensory physiology of this species. Here, we analyzed the visual response spectrum of live L. laeta specimens by complementary electrophysiological and behavioral experiments. Given their mostly nocturnal habits, we hypothesized that they present a visual spectral range similar to the secondary eyes of other nocturnal spiders (Barth et al., 1993), and prefer darker versus brighter places to dwell.

Accordingly, we observed electroretinogram (ERG) and behavioral responses to light in the range from ultraviolet (UV) to green wavelengths. Surprisingly, an increased exploratory behavior of illuminated versus dark areas was evident in the open field test. These findings provide data on a public health-relevant species that will allow a better understanding of its behavior and possibly the development of strategies to reduce human exposure to its bites.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Loxosceles laeta (Nicolet 1849) (Fig. 1A) individuals were supplied by students of the Faculty of Science of the University of Valparaíso, as they are present in the majority of homes in central Chile. The spiders were kept in 100 ml plastic containers adapted for adequate ventilation under a natural photoperiod at 20–24°C and 60–80% relative humidity. Specimens were staged by size according to the description by Canals and Solís (2014), and further classified by sex based on visual inspection of the pedipalps. Twelve animals (6 females and 6 males) were used for ERG recordings and 8 animals (6 females and 2 males) for behavioral tests. The mass (mean±s.d.) of the spiders was 69.65±39.7 mg for females and 75.77±34.57 mg for males, and the average leg length was 2±0.37 cm for females and 2.45±0.42 cm for males. Before the experiments, the specimens were anesthetized with CO2 in a closed plastic container for a period of 3 min at room temperature. This allowed safe manipulation of the animals during the procedures.

Histology

The legs and abdomen were removed from the cephalothorax, which was fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde solution for 24 h at 4°C. For cryosections, the tissue was cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in PBS for 48 h, embedded in tissue freezing medium (Tissue Tek, Sakura, Mountain View, CA, USA) and cut in a cryostat (Leica CM 1900, Berlin, Germany) at −20°C into 18 µm sections, which were stained in 1% Toluidine Blue (Sigma-Aldrich, MO, USA) for 1 min. Alternatively, fixed tissue was dehydrated in a graded series of ethanol and acetone and embedded in Eponate (Pelco, Fresno, CA, USA). Semi-thin sections (1 µm) were cut in an ultramicrotome (Reichert, Austria) and stained with 1% Toluidine Blue for 2 min.

ERG recordings

Anesthetized animals were placed inside a Petri dish and the legs were kept in position using two paper strips fastened with masking tape (Fig. 2A). The dish was covered with 5% type VII-A agarose (Sigma-Aldrich), leaving the prosoma exposed. Under this configuration, animals remained immobile for the duration of the experiments, avoiding the necessity of further anesthesia. The animal was then placed on a rotatory platform under a stereomicroscope, which allowed us to position different pairs of eyes towards the stimulation source. A fiberoptic light guide (model 77526, Newport, Irvine, CA, USA) was placed 2 cm away from the recorded eye, and the correct positioning of the light source was verified using the reflection of the light on the tapetum (Fig. 2B). The light guide was connected to one of the LEDs in a custom array with emission spectra spanning from UV to infrared (IR) wavelengths. The spectral characteristics for each LED were measured with a calibrated fiberoptic spectrometer (Ocean Optics USB4000, Largo, FL, USA), and are detailed in Table S1. The LED array was controlled using custom software, and the irradiance of each individual LED was calibrated using three repeated measurements with an optical power meter (1918-R, Newport). ERG recordings were obtained using tungsten electrodes with an impedance of about 13 MΩ (FHC, Bowdoin, ME, USA) connected to a differential amplifier (Model 3000, A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA, USA) used in differential and AC mode (band-pass filtered between 1 and 100 Hz, gain 10k). Despite the unstable baseline, several ERG recordings in DC mode were also obtained (Fig. 3A), which confirmed the proportionality of AC versus DC amplitude measurements (Fig. S1). Recordings were digitized using an analog-to-digital converter card (PCI-6221 with chassis BNC-2090, National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) controlled by WinWCP 4.9.4 software (University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK). A recording electrode was inserted through the junction between the lens and the chitin covering the cephalothorax (Fig. 2C); a reference electrode was placed in the anterior trochanter of the first leg ipsilateral to the recorded eye (Fig. 2D), and a ground electrode was inserted into the agarose. The three right eyes were recorded, one for each eye pair. To establish the amount of time necessary for dark adaptation prior to the ERG recordings, a series of 500 ms green (533 nm) light flashes were presented at increasing time intervals until the amplitude of the response reached a plateau. The data were fitted to an exponential function using the least square method to obtain the maximum steady-state response amplitude (Fig. 3B). The calculated amount of time to reach 99% of the maximum steady-state response amplitude value was 8.79±0.13 min (N=3); therefore, the animals were dark adapted for a period of 15 min before the experiments.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Methodological overview. (A) For immobilization, the legs of the animal were held in place using paper strips fastened with masking tape and covered with agarose, leaving the body exposed. (B) The reflection in the tapetum was used to determine a correct alignment of the fiberoptic light guide before electrode placement (C). (D) Schematic representation of the electroretinogram (ERG) setup. The recording electrode (RecE) is placed in the cornea, the reference electrode (RefE) is in the trochanter of the first ipsilateral leg and a fiberoptic light guide (FO) is positioned in front of the eye and connected to one of the LEDs of the custom array. (E) Schematic representation of the behavioral assay setup. A box covered in black cardboard was used for open field tests. A small window was opened on each side of the box and a stimulator containing a LED and a variable neutral density filter to control the intensity of the light was placed adjacent to one of the openings. A camera was placed above the box to record the behavior of the animal. (F) From the inside of the box, the setup produced a discrete square area of illumination. The image corresponds to a frame from a representative video recording.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Spectral sensitivity of the three eye pairs of L. laeta. (A) Representative ERG recording showing a cornea-negative potential as a response to a 499 nm light stimulus. Analyses were performed using the peak amplitude of the response. Bar, stimulation period; filled arrowhead, peak response amplitude in AC mode; open arrowhead, peak response amplitude in DC mode. (B) Representative plot of response amplitudes during dark adaptation, fitted to an exponential curve. (C) Normalized average spectral sensitivities for the three eye pairs of L. laeta (means±s.e.m., n=6). The colors correspond to each eye pair as shown in the inset [blue, posterior median (PM); orange, anterior lateral (AL); gray, posterior lateral (PL)]. Dashed lines represent the pigment template curves fitted to the data as described by Stavenga (2010). (D) Comparison of the normalized average spectral sensitivity for male (dark blue) and female (light blue) individuals for the PM eyes (means±s.e.m., n=6 per sex, n=12). No significant differences between the sexes were observed. (E) Top: sensitivity curves for chromatic adaptation experiments, expressed as the log of the quantal sensitivity. Quantal sensitivity is defined as the inverse of the quanta required to elicit a criterion response. Each adaptation light is shown in a different color, with the dark-adapted condition shown in black (means±s.e.m., n=6 per condition). Bottom: the normalized spectral characteristics of each adaptation light source, shown using the same colors as above.

To construct the spectral sensitivity curves, monochromatic 500 ms test flashes were presented to the eye, starting from the minimum intensity available and increasing until a criterion response was achieved. This criterion response was set to 20 μV above the baseline noise (average noise 22.6±1.4 μV) and was determined using the on-line analysis tool of WinWCP 4.9.4. This protocol was then repeated two more times, for a total of three recordings for each LED available in the array. Each test flash was separated by a period of 10 s. The spectral sensitivity was obtained as the inverse of the quantal intensity necessary to produce the criterion response. The same protocol was repeated to test selective monochromatic adaptation. To this end, the animals were first dark adapted for 15 min, and then adapted to UV (380 nm), green (533 nm) or long wavelength light (approximately 490–790 nm; white LED with long wavelength filter) using LEDs connected to the eyepiece of the stereomicroscope for another 15 min. The adapting light was kept on for the duration of the experiments, except for a period of 500 ms prior to and after each test flash. At the end of the ERG recordings, the animals were anesthetized again, removed from the agarose and returned to their containers.

Behavioral assays

The apparatus used for open field tests was a custom-built 30×30 cm glass box, the walls of which were covered on the outside using black card stock and on the inside with polytetrafluoroethylene strips to prevent the animals from climbing out (Fig. 2E). Each of the four sides of the box had a 1×2 cm rectangular hole in the bottom center of the cardboard. At the start of each test, one of the four sides of the box was chosen using a random number generator (https://www.random.org/), and a stimulator containing a LED and a variable neutral density filter (ND 2-400 filter, Neewer, Shenzhen, PRC) was placed facing the hole in the wall from the outside, at a distance of 1 cm and an angle of 30 deg downwards. Seen from the inside of the box, this produced a discrete square area of illumination (Fig. 2F). The LED was turned on and the animals (under light-adapted conditions) were released in the middle of the box and allowed to freely explore the inside for 10 min. The procedure was repeated for the following wavelengths: violet, 422 nm; blue, 457 nm; green, 517 nm; red, 635 nm; and IR, 855 nm. Negative controls were performed without light stimulation. The intensity of the different LEDs was equated to the LED with the least brightness – in this case, the violet LED – using the variable density filter and calibrated with an optical power meter (1918-R, Newport). The target intensity in radiant flux was 1.14×1016 photons s−1 cm−2. The behavior of the animal was recorded immediately after its release using a webcam (C920 HD Pro, Logitech) located above the apparatus at a constant frame rate of 2 frames s−1, using a dim white background illumination (44 lx in the center, 28±5 lx at the edges). Between trials, the inside of the box was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol. Each wavelength was tested on separate days. For analysis, the open area was divided into nine sectors, with four sectors comprising the corners of the field (sectors 1, 3, 7 and 9), four sectors comprising the open sides of the walls (sectors 2, 4, 6 and 8) and one central sector (sector 5) (Fig. 4A).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Open field test. (A–F) Left, box plots showing the time spent in each of the side sectors as a percentage of total time, with the illuminated sector marked as Stim, and the other three side sectors counted clockwise (side numbers in A correspond to those shown on the right). Right, heat map showing a representative path of a single animal (blue line); the colors represent the amount of time spent in each position (as the logarithm of the number of video frames over the total number of video frames). The test area was divided into nine sectors, which were then grouped into corner sectors (1, 3, 7 and 9), side sectors (2, 4, 6 and 8) and a central sector (5), as shown in A. The stimulation sector is marked with a square in B–F. Data are shown for the control condition (no test light; A), and using the violet (422 nm; B), blue (457 nm; C), green (517 nm; D), red (635 nm; E) and infrared (855 nm; F) LEDs (n=8 per condition, Kruskall–Wallis test followed by Dunn's multiple comparisons test, *P<0.05). For box plots, bars represent the medians, boxes are the interquartile range and whiskers are the data range; circles are outliers.

Data analysis

ERG recordings were stored and analyzed offline using WinWCP 4.9.4 software (University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK). Sensitivity curves were first normalized per individual and then averaged between individuals using custom software written in Python 3.7.1. The visual pigment template described by Stavenga (2010) was fitted to the green sensitivity peak using the least squares method via the curve_fit function from the open source SciPy 1.3.1 package. The video recordings were analyzed offline with a modified version of a custom-programmed tracking algorithm previously used by the laboratory (Calbiague et al., 2017), which is available online at https://sourceforge.net/projects/spider-tracking/. Statistical analysis was performed using Stata 13.0 software (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). The data were first analyzed for normal distribution using the Shapiro–Wilk test. In all cases, at least one variable was found not to conform to a normal distribution, so significant differences were established with the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn's multiple comparisons test. For all tests, the α value was set to 0.05. Unless otherwise stated, data are presented as means±s.e.m. Graphics were constructed with custom software written in Python 3.7.1, using the open source MatPlotLib 3.0.3 package.

RESULTS

Histological analysis

Transverse semi-thin sections and cryosections through the median eyes of L. laeta revealed a simple structure with a one-tiered retina and photoreceptor rhabdoms oriented towards the reflective tapetum (Fig. 1C,D), consistent with the secondary spider eye type (Foelix, 2011). This is in line with the notion that all eyes of sicariids are secondary eyes, the primary eyes being absent (Barth, 2002b; Schmidt-Rhaesa et al., 2015), and the central eyes corresponding to displaced PM eyes (Gertsch and Ennik, 1983). We estimate that the eyes of L. laeta each contain about 100 photoreceptors.

ERG recordings

The ERG responses obtained corresponded to monophasic cornea-negative potentials (Fig. 3A), similar to the ones already described in other spider species (Barth et al., 1993; Defrize et al., 2011), but with comparatively low amplitudes in the microvolt range. All three eye types displayed a main sensitivity peak around 500 nm and another one extending into the UV range, past the limit of our equipment (Fig. 3C). When fitted using a rhodopsin template as described by Stavenga (2010), the obtained peaks were 534, 530 and 525 nm for the PM, AL and PL eyes, respectively (R2 values: PM, 0.97; AL, 0.97; PL, 0.98). The values for the green sensitivity peak are similar to that measured for spider rhodopsin-1 (Rh1), recently cloned and described in jumping spiders (Koyanagi et al., 2008; Nagata et al., 2012). The spectral sensitivity plots showed similar characteristics for all eye types, and no statistically significant differences were found between the three eye pairs. These results suggest that no spectral specialization exists between the three secondary eye pairs. As no differences were found between eyes of the same animals, possible spectral sensitivity specializations of male or female specimens were analyzed. To this end, averaged sensitivity curves from the PM eyes of both sexes were compared, but neither the PM eyes (Fig. 3D) nor the PL and AL eyes displayed any statistically significant differences (Fig. S2). These results argue against the presence of sex-specific visual specializations at the retinal level.

Finally, to test whether the observed spectral sensitivity peaks correspond to two or more different visual pigments, spectral sensitivity curves were constructed under chromatic adaptation to UV, green and long wavelength light. While the overall sensitivity decreased under each type of adaptation by up to two log units compared with dark-adapted conditions, no significant difference was observed between the three adaptation curves in their overall sensitivity level (Fig. 3E). When comparing the ratio between the green and UV sensitivity peaks, a significant difference was found between the dark-adapted condition and the different chromatic adaptations, but no differences were found between each chromatic adaptation (dark, 1.02±0.17; UV, 6.92±1.84; green, 7.27±0.6; long, 5.18±2.07; dark-adapted included, P=0.03; dark-adapted excluded P=0.74), arguing against the possibility of more than one functionally independent light-sensitive pigment in the eyes of L. laeta.

Behavioral tests

To further characterize the visual system of L. laeta and to obtain a behavioral correlate to our electrophysiological data, we tested the preference for different wavelengths using an open field test. Open field tests are widely used to evaluate exploratory behavior, and have been previously used with spiders (Baatrup and Bayley, 1993; Carducci and Jakob, 2000). When exposed to our open field, the specimens showed a highly thigmotactic behavior, exploring the borders of the field and avoiding the central sector (Fig. 4). In negative controls (without light stimulus), the spiders preferred the corner sectors over the open side sectors (corners, 87.1±2.9%; sides, 12.2±2.5%; center, 0.7±0.5%; P=0.0001). No preference was observed for any of the open side sectors (side 2, 33.2±5.8%; side 6, 21.3±6.1%; side 8, 25.4±6.3%; side 4, 20.2±6.5%; P=0.3) (Fig. 4A). For this reason, the remainder of the tests were analyzed comparing the open side sectors only. The overall activity level of the spiders was assessed using the number of sector boundary crossings. There were no significant differences between conditions (violet, 39.8±4.4; blue, 42±7.6; green, 31.9±5.9; red, 31.5±6; IR, 45.6±9; control, 18±2.4; P=0.39).

To test the behavioral spectral preference, we added a light source to one of the sides of the open field, producing a discrete illuminated zone in one of the open side sectors. For analysis, we defined a stimulation sector (Stim), and numbered the rest of the open side sectors clockwise. Surprisingly, the presence of the light source caused the spiders to increase the time exploring the illuminated sectors. This effect was clearly observed when using the violet (Stim, 45.9±6.2%; side 1, 21±5.2%; side 2, 16.6±2.5%; side 3, 16.4±2.6%; P=0.0014) (Fig. 4B), blue (Stim, 42.6±5.4%; side 1, 23.7±3.8%; side 2, 17.6±2.3%; side 3, 16.1±4.4%; P=0.0075) (Fig. 4C) and green lights (Stim, 45.3±5.7; % side 1, 22.4±4.1%; side 2, 19.8±5.9%; side 3, 12.4±2.9%; P=0.0025) (Fig. 4D), with an increase of the time spent in the illuminated side sector compared with the other sectors. The effect was abolished when using the red (Stim, 24.3±4.4%; side 1, 26±7.2%; side 2, 19.8±3.9%; side 3, 30±7.3%; P=0.88) (Fig. 4E) and IR lights (Stim, 16.1±3.2%; side 1, 26.1±6.1%; side 2, 30±5.5%; side 3, 27.8±6.8%; P=0.3) (Fig. 4F). These results are in line with the electrophysiological data, showing that L. laeta has a spectral sensitivity spanning from the UV to the green spectrum that decreases towards longer wavelengths, without any apparent IR sensitivity.

DISCUSSION

We used the flash ERG technique to measure the spectral sensitivity of the eyes of L. laeta. The data obtained show responses between the UV (370 nm) and red (650 nm) parts of the spectrum, which is consistent with previous studies in different genera, including Cupiennius (Barth et al., 1993; Walla et al., 1996), Lycosa (DeVoe, 1972; DeVoe et al., 1969), Deinopis (Laughlin et al., 1980), Leucorchestris (Norgaard et al., 2008) and Misumena (Defrize et al., 2011). The ERG amplitudes obtained, generally in the 25–100 µV range under dark-adapted conditions, were rather low compared with those of the aforementioned studies, which may be due to the small eye size of L. laeta or to a low shunting resistance of the ERG circuit that reduced the response amplitude while preserving the signal-to-noise ratio. Based on the shape of the spectral sensitivity plots obtained, the sensitivity range probably extends deeper into the UV range, but this was not testable with our equipment. Importantly, there were no significant differences between the sensitivity spectra of the three eye pairs, suggesting that there is no spectral specialization between eye types in this spider species, as opposed to others (Defrize et al., 2011; DeVoe et al., 1969). However, different kinds of eye type-specific specializations, like motion or polarization detection, as described for other species (Mueller and Labhart, 2010; Neuhofer et al., 2009), cannot be excluded.

The sensitivity spectra observed in L. laeta are consistent with the expression of the bistable spider Rh1 (Nagata et al., 2012; Varma et al., 2019), responsible for the peak in the green range, but the marked sensitivity peak in the UV range remains to be explained. One potential mechanism is the presence of an accessory sensitizing pigment absorbing in the UV range. The presence and function of a sensitizing pigment, presumably 3-hydroxyretinol, has been reported for several insect species (Kirschfeld and Vogt, 1986; Stavenga et al., 2017). A related vitamin A derivative, 11-cis-retinol, was detected by HPLC in the eyes of C. salei (Barth et al., 1993), but the presence of 3-hydroxyretinol was not reported and, to our knowledge, no other studies have investigated the possible role of UV-absorbing sensitizing pigments in spider photoreceptors. A more likely possibility is that the UV peak of L. laeta corresponds to an enhanced β-band of Rh1. Indeed, the absorption spectra of invertebrate rhodopsins were recently shown to depend strongly on the counter-ions provided by amino acids close to the chromophore (Nagata et al., 2019); thus, a variant of Rh1 might be responsible for the observed spectrum.

After chromatic preadaptation with UV, green and long wavelength light, the overall sensitivity was significantly reduced when compared with the dark-adapted condition. We did not observe significant differences between the preadapted conditions in terms of overall sensitivity and relative sensitivity between the green and UV peaks, which is not compatible with the expression of more than one type of rhodopsin in the same or different photoreceptors, although we cannot fully exclude the possibility of the expression of two rhodopsin pigments with similar peak absorption in the green range, as reported in C. salei (Walla et al., 1996). Therefore, despite its two spectral sensitivity peaks, L. laeta is likely to have only monochromatic vision. Prior studies in spiders have found evidence for differential rhodopsin expression in photoreceptors, and two or three sensitivity peaks in ERG recordings in a few model species, but evidence for dichromatic or trichromatic color vision in arachnids remains scarce (Barth, 2002a; Foelix, 2011; Morehouse et al., 2017; Walla et al., 1996; Zurek et al., 2015). For example, the wandering spider Leucorchestris arenicola displayed only one peak in the green spectrum in all eye types (Norgaard et al., 2008). Spectral sensitivity analysis using ERG recordings revealed two peaks, in the UV and green range, in all eye types of the crab spider M. vatia (Defrize et al., 2011), similar to our results. Different wolf spider (Lycosa) species showed two similar peaks in the AM eyes, while their secondary eyes only had one green sensitivity peak (DeVoe et al., 1969). In contrast, differential expression of UV- and green-sensitive opsins in photoreceptors of principal and secondary eyes was found in specimens of the genera Cupiennius, Phidippus, Lycosa and Thomisus, using intracellular recording of photoreceptors (Defrize et al., 2011; DeVoe, 1972, 1975; Walla et al., 1996). In C. salei, initial ERG studies revealed the presence of a main green peak (520–540 nm) and a UV shoulder (340–380 nm), but selective chromatic adaptation failed to indicate the existence of more than one type of photoreceptor (Barth et al., 1993). Later studies using intracellular recordings suggested the presence of three different photoreceptor types, with sensitivity maxima in the blue (480 nm), green (520 nm) and UV (340 nm) for all eyes (Walla et al., 1996). The same authors also noted the possibility of more than one visual pigment per photoreceptor, as seen in other arthropods (Sakamoto et al., 1996) and proposed for Lycosa (DeVoe, 1972), or the existence of electrical coupling between different photoreceptor cells (Walla et al., 1996). Yet, a newer study showed that, when tested using moving color stripes over backgrounds of varying gray levels, C. salei individuals were unable to discriminate the blue-, green- and red-colored stimuli from certain gray levels (Orlando and Schmid, 2011). The authors concluded that even though C. salei has three types of photoreceptors, the secondary eyes, the ones in charge of movement detection, are apparently color blind (Orlando and Schmid, 2011).

In order to further characterize the effect of chromatic stimulation on the behavior of L. laeta, it was necessary to develop a specific paradigm, as assays involving the movement of the retina, such as those described by Orlando and Schmid (2011), were not possible because of the lack of retinal muscles, and other behavioral paradigms, such as the one used by Zopf et al. (2013) involving the search for a black target, did not elicit any directed response in L. laeta. Thus, the physiological sensitivity spectrum obtained here by ERG recordings was complemented by behavioral data obtained with a modified open field test, in which the animals were exposed to discrete illuminated areas in specific sections of their path. The observation of a significantly longer exploration time of areas illuminated by light in the violet to green range, without reaction to red and near-IR light, might be explained by a higher overall sensitivity of L. laeta to short wavelength as opposed to longer wavelength light, which is in line with our electrophysiological findings, and does not necessarily indicate a preference for short wavelength light.

Interestingly, the available evidence classifies this species as a nocturnal animal (Canals et al., 2015a), but our behavioral assays showed no avoidance of illuminated areas, and instead an increased exploration time of these compared with unilluminated side areas, although the exact significance of the observed exploratory behavior remains to be studied. It is of note, however, that the (darker) corners of the maze remained most attractive to the animals, in accordance with their local name of araña de rincón (‘corner spider’).

Efforts are underway to assemble the genome of Loxosceles reclusa (Brown Recluse Spider Genome Project, https://www.hgsc.bcm.edu/arthropods/brown-recluse-spider-genome-project). This will reveal whether a variant of jumping spider Rh1 is indeed present in the Loxosceles genome, and whether other types of opsins are also present. Further studies are clearly needed to better understand the visual systems, and visually driven behaviors, of this and other spider species.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Dr Alex Vielma for a critical revision of the manuscript and advice on ERG recordings, Dr Adrián Palacios for access to the ERG stimulation apparatus and optical power meter, and Dr Nicolás Palanca for helpful advice.

FOOTNOTES

  • Competing interests

    The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

  • Author contributions

    Conceptualization: O.S., F.T., J.O.; Methodology: F.T.; Software: F.T.; Validation: F.T.; Formal analysis: F.T.; Investigation: O.S., F.T., J.O.; Resources: O.S.; Data curation: F.T.; Writing - original draft: O.S., F.T., J.O.; Writing - review & editing: O.S., F.T., J.O.; Supervision: O.S.; Project administration: O.S.; Funding acquisition: O.S.

  • Funding

    This study was supported by FONDECYT (grant no. 1171228), CONICYT PhD scholarship (F.T.) and the Millennium Institute Interdisciplinary Center of Neuroscience of Valparaíso (CINV: grant no. ICM P09-022-F).

  • Supplementary information

    Supplementary information available online at http://jeb.biologists.org/lookup/doi/10.1242/jeb.217133.supplemental

  • Received October 22, 2019.
  • Accepted December 10, 2019.
  • © 2020. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd
http://www.biologists.com/user-licence-1-1/

References

  1. ↵
    1. Baatrup, E. and
    2. Bayley, M.
    (1993). Quantitative analysis of spider locomotion employing computer-automated video tracking. Physiol. Behav. 54, 83-90. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(93)90047-J
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Barth, F. G.
    (2002a). Spider senses - technical perfection and biology. Zoology 105, 271-285. doi:10.1078/0944-2006-00082
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  3. ↵
    1. Barth, F. G.
    (2002b). A Spider's World. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.
  4. ↵
    1. Barth, F.,
    2. Nakagawa, T. and
    3. Eguchi, E.
    (1993). Vision in the ctenid spider Cupiennius salei: spectral range and absolute sensitivity. J. Exp. Biol. 181, 63-79.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Binford, G. J. and
    2. Wells, M. A.
    (2003). The phylogenetic distribution of sphingomyelinase D activity in venoms of Haplogyne spiders. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B Biochem. Mol. Biol. 135, 25-33. doi:10.1016/S1096-4959(03)00045-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    1. Blest, A. D.
    (1985). The fine structure of spider photoreceptors in relation to function. In Neurobiology of Arachnids (ed. F. G. Barth), pp. 79-102. Berlin: Springer.
  7. ↵
    1. Calbiague, V. M.,
    2. Olivares, J.,
    3. Olivares, E. and
    4. Schmachtenberg, O.
    (2017). The Chilean Recluse Spider (Araneae: Sicariidae) displays behavioral responses to conspecific odors, but not to several general odorants. J. Med. Entomol. 54, 1435-1439. doi:10.1093/jme/tjx101
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Canals, M. and
    2. Solís, R.
    (2014). Desarrollo de cohortes y parámetros poblacionales de la araña del rincón Loxosceles laeta. Rev. Chil. Infectología 31, 555-562. doi:10.4067/S0716-10182014000500007
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    1. Canals, M.,
    2. Veloso, C. and
    3. Solís, R.
    (2015a). Adaptation of the spiders to the environment: The case of some Chilean species. Front. Physiol. 6, 1-9. doi:10.3389/fphys.2015.00220
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Canals, M.,
    2. Arriagada, N. and
    3. Solis, R.
    (2015b). Interactions between the chilean recluse spider (Araneae: Sicariidae) and an araneophagic spitting spider (Araneae: Scytodidae). J. Med. Entomol. 52, 109-116. doi:10.1093/jme/tju021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Canals, M.,
    2. Canals, M. J. and
    3. Tacaure-Rios, A.
    (2015c). Estimation of the potential distribution of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta and the spitting spider Scytodes globula from preferred temperatures in the laboratory. Parasitol. Latinoam. 64, 22-29. doi:10.1016/j.jtherbio.2013.08.003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    1. Carducci, J. P. and
    2. Jakob, E. M.
    (2000). Rearing environment affects behaviour of jumping spiders. Anim. Behav. 59, 39-46. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1282
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Chaves-Moreira, D.,
    2. Senff-Ribeiro, A.,
    3. Wille, A. C. M.,
    4. Gremski, L. H.,
    5. Chaim, O. M. and
    6. Veiga, S. S.
    (2017). Highlights in the knowledge of brown spider toxins. J. Venom. Anim. Toxins Incl. Trop. Dis. 23, 1-12. doi:10.1186/s40409-017-0097-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Cross, F. R.,
    2. Jackson, R. R. and
    3. Pollard, S. D.
    (2008). Complex display behaviour of Evarcha culicivora, an East African mosquito-eating jumping spider. New Zeal. J. Zool. 35, 151-187. doi:10.1080/03014220809510112
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Dacke, M.,
    2. Doan, T. A. and
    3. O'Carroll, D. C.
    (2001). Polarized light detection in spiders. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2481-2490.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Defrize, J.,
    2. Lazzari, C. R.,
    3. Warrant, E. J. and
    4. Casas, J.
    (2011). Spectral sensitivity of a colour changing spider. J. Insect Physiol. 57, 508-513. doi:10.1016/j.jinsphys.2011.01.016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. DeVoe, R. D.
    (1972). Dual sensitivities of cells in wolf spider eyes at ultraviolet and visible wavelengths of light. J. Gen. Physiol. 59, 247-269. doi:10.1085/jgp.59.3.247
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. DeVoe, R. D.
    (1975). Ultraviolet and green receptors in principal eyes of jumping spiders. J. Gen. Physiol. 66, 193-207. doi:10.1085/jgp.66.2.193
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. DeVoe, R. D.,
    2. Small, R. J. W. and
    3. Zvargulis, J. E.
    (1969). Spectral sensitivities of wolf spider Eyes. J. Gen. Physiol. 54, 1-32. doi:10.1085/jgp.54.1.1
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Elliott, W. R.,
    2. Reddell, J. R.,
    3. Rudolph, D. C.,
    4. Graening, G. O.,
    5. Briggs, T. S.,
    6. Ubick, D.,
    7. Aalbu, R. L.,
    8. Krejca, J. K. and
    9. Taylor, S. J.
    (2017). The Cave Fauna of California. Proc. Calif. Acad. Sci. 64, 1-311.
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    1. Foelix, R. F.
    (2011). Biology of Spiders, 3rd edn. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
  22. ↵
    1. George, W. and
    2. Peckham, E. G.
    (1887). Some observations on the mental powers of spiders. J. Morphol. 1, 383-419. doi:10.1002/jmor.1050010206
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Gertsch, W. J. and
    2. Ennik, F.
    (1983). The spider genus Loxosceles in North America, Central America, and the West Indies (Aranae, Loxoscelidae). Bull. Am. Museum Nat. Hist. 175, 264-360. doi:10.1016/0041-0101(85)90178-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Gonçalves-de-Andrade, R. M.,
    2. Petrel, F. D. and
    3. Tambourgi, D. V.
    (2007). The spider Loxosceles adelaida Gerstch, 1967 (Araneae, Sicariidae) in the karstic area of Ribeira Valley, PETAR, Sao Paulo, Brazil. J. Entomol. 4, 46-50. doi:10.3923/je.2007.46.50
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Jackson, R. R.,
    2. Nelson, X. J. and
    3. Sune, G. O.
    (2005). A spider that feeds indirectly on vertebrate blood by choosing female mosquitoes as prey. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 15155-15160. doi:10.1073/pnas.0507398102
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Kirschfeld, K. and
    2. Vogt, K.
    (1986). Does retinol serve a sensitizing function in insect photoreceptors? Vis. Res 26, 1771-1777. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(86)90127-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Koyanagi, M.,
    2. Nagata, T.,
    3. Katoh, K.,
    4. Yamashita, S. and
    5. Tokunaga, F.
    (2008). Molecular evolution of arthropod color vision deduced from multiple opsin genes of jumping spiders. J. Mol. Evol. 66, 130-137. doi:10.1007/s00239-008-9065-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  28. ↵
    1. Land, M. F. and
    2. Nilsson, D.-E.
    (2012). Animal Eyes, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  29. ↵
    1. Laughlin, S.,
    2. Blest, A. D. and
    3. Stowe, S.
    (1980). The sensitivity of receptors in the posterior median eye of the nocturnal spider, Dinopis. J. Comp. Physiol. A 141, 53-65. doi:10.1007/BF00611878
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. ↵
    1. Menda, G.,
    2. Shamble, P. S.,
    3. Nitzany, E. I.,
    4. Golden, J. R. and
    5. Hoy, R. R.
    (2014). Visual perception in the brain of a jumping spider. Curr. Biol. 24, 2580-2585. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Morehouse, N. I.,
    2. Buschbeck, E. K.,
    3. Zurek, D. B.,
    4. Steck, M. and
    5. Porter, M. L.
    (2017). Molecular evolution of spider vision: New opportunities, familiar players. Biol. Bull. 233, 21-38. doi:10.1086/693977
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    1. Mueller, K. P. and
    2. Labhart, T.
    (2010). Polarizing optics in a spider eye. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 196, 335-348. doi:10.1007/s00359-010-0516-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Nagata, T.,
    2. Koyanagi, M.,
    3. Tsukamoto, H.,
    4. Saeki, S.,
    5. Isono, K.,
    6. Shichida, Y.,
    7. Tokunaga, F.,
    8. Kinoshita, M.,
    9. Arikawa, K. and
    10. Terkita, A.
    (2012). Depth perception from image defocus. Science 335, 469-472. doi:10.1126/science.1211667
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Nagata, T.,
    2. Koyanagi, M.,
    3. Tsukamoto, H.,
    4. Mutt, E.,
    5. Schertler, G. F. X.,
    6. Deupi, X. and
    7. Terakita, A.
    (2019). The counterion–retinylidene Schiff base interaction of an invertebrate rhodopsin rearranges upon light activation. Commun. Biol. 2, 1-9. doi:10.1038/s42003-019-0409-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    1. Nentwig, W.
    (2013). Spider Ecophysiology. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.
  36. ↵
    1. Neuhofer, D.,
    2. Machan, R. and
    3. Schmid, A.
    (2009). Visual perception of motion in a hunting spider. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 2819-2823. doi:10.1242/jeb.027136
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Norgaard, T.,
    2. Henschel, J. R.,
    3. Garm, A.,
    4. Wehner, R. and
    5. Nilsson, D.-E.
    (2008). Vision in the nocturnal wandering spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae: Sparassidae). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 816-823. doi:10.1242/jeb.010546
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Nuboer, J. F. W.
    (1986). A comparative view on color vision. Netherlands J. Zool. 3, 344-380. doi:10.1163/002829686X00126
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Orlando, E. and
    2. Schmid, A.
    (2011). Colour blindness of the movement-detecting system of the spider Cupiennius salei. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 546-550. doi:10.1242/jeb.051672
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Sakamoto, K.,
    2. Hisatomi, O.,
    3. Tokunaga, F. and
    4. Eguchi, E.
    (1996). Two opsins from the compound eye of the crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 441-450.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Schmidt-Rhaesa, A.,
    2. Harzsch, S. and
    3. Purschke, G.
    (2015). Structure and Evolution of Invertebrate Nervous Systems. Oxford University Press.
  42. ↵
    1. Stavenga, D. G.
    (2010). On visual pigment templates and the spectral shape of invertebrate rhodopsins and metarhodopsins. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 196, 869-878. doi:10.1007/s00359-010-0568-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Stavenga, D. G.,
    2. Wehling, M. F. and
    3. Belušič, G.
    (2017). Functional interplay of visual, sensitizing and screening pigments in the eyes of Drosophila and other red-eyed dipteran flies. J. Physiol. 595, 5481-5494. doi:10.1113/JP273674
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  44. ↵
    1. Tacaure-Ríos, A.
    (2011). Loxosceles surca (Gertsch, 1967) (Araneae: Sicariidae) en el norte de Chile. Boletín Biodivers. Chile 5, 45-49.
  45. ↵
    1. Varma, N.,
    2. Mutt, E.,
    3. Mühle, J.,
    4. Panneels, V.,
    5. Terakita, A.,
    6. Deupi, X.,
    7. Nogly, P.,
    8. Schertler, G. F. X. and
    9. Lesca, E.
    (2019). Crystal structure of jumping spider rhodopsin-1 as a light sensitive GPCR. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 116, 14547-14556. doi:10.1073/pnas.1902192116
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Walla, P.,
    2. Barth, F. G. and
    3. Eguchi, E.
    (1996). Spectral sensitivity of single photoreceptor cells in the eyes of the ctenid spicier Cupiennius salei keys. Zoolog. Sci. 13, 199-202. doi:10.2108/zsj.13.199
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Yamashita, S.
    (1985). Photoreceptor cells in the spider eye: spectral sensitivity and efferent control. In Neurobiology of Arachnids (ed. F. Barth), pp. 103-117. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
  48. ↵
    1. Zopf, L. M.,
    2. Schmid, A.,
    3. Fredman, D. and
    4. Eriksson, B. J.
    (2013). Spectral sensitivity of the ctenid spider Cupiennius salei. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 4103-4108. doi:10.1242/jeb.086256
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Zurek, D. B.,
    2. Cronin, T. W.,
    3. Taylor, L. A.,
    4. Byrne, K.,
    5. Sullivan, M. L. G. and
    6. Morehouse, N. I.
    (2015). Spectral filtering enables trichromatic vision in colorful jumping spiders. Curr. Biol. 25, R403-R404. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • Arthropods
  • Eye
  • Retina
  • Ultraviolet vision
  • Electroretinogram
  • Open field test

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The visual spectral sensitivity of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
The visual spectral sensitivity of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta
Felipe Tapia, Jesús Olivares, Oliver Schmachtenberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb217133 doi: 10.1242/jeb.217133 Published 22 January 2020
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
The visual spectral sensitivity of the Chilean recluse spider Loxosceles laeta
Felipe Tapia, Jesús Olivares, Oliver Schmachtenberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2020 223: jeb217133 doi: 10.1242/jeb.217133 Published 22 January 2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgements
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Unexpected lack of specialisation in the flow properties of spitting cobra venom
  • Responses of Manduca sexta larvae to heat waves
  • Heat hardening in a pair of Anolis lizards: constraints, dynamics and ecological consequences
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLE

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Predicting the Future: Species Survival in a Changing World

Read our new special issue exploring the significant role of experimental biology in assessing and predicting the susceptibility or resilience of species to future, human-induced environmental change.


Big Biology Podcast - Hollie Putnam and coral bleaching

Catch the next JEB-sponsored episode of the Big Biology Podcast where Art and Marty talk to Hollie Putnam about the causes of coral bleaching and the basic biology of corals in the hope of selectively breeding corals that can better tolerate future ocean conditions.

Read Hollie's Review on the subject, which is featured in our current special issue. 


Stark trade-offs and elegant solutions in arthropod visual systems

Many elegant eye specializations that evolved in response to visual challenges continue to be discovered. A new Review by Meece et al. summarises exciting solutions evolved by insects and other arthropods in response to specific visual challenges.


Head bobbing gives pigeons a sense of perspective

Pigeons might look goofy with their head-bobbing walk, but it turns out that the ungainly head manoeuvre allows the birds to judge distance.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992