Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
COMMENTARY
Requiem for a heavyweight – can anything more be learned from homing pigeons about the sensory and spatial-representational basis of avian navigation?
Verner P. Bingman
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb163089 doi: 10.1242/jeb.163089 Published 22 October 2018
Verner P. Bingman
Department of Psychology and J. P. Scott Center for Neuroscience, Mind and Behavior, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Verner P. Bingman
  • For correspondence: vbingma@bgsu.edu
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

The homing pigeon (Columba livia) has long served as a study species to exhaustively investigate the sensory and spatial (map)-representational mechanisms that guide avian navigation. However, several factors have contributed to recent questioning of whether homing pigeons are as valuable as they once were as a general model for the study of the sensory and map-like, spatial-representational mechanisms of avian navigation. These reservations include: the success of this research program in unveiling navigational mechanisms; the burgeoning of new tracking technologies making navigational experiments on long-distance migratory and other wild birds much more accessible; the almost complete loss of the historically dominant, large-scale pigeon loft/research facilities; and prohibitive university per diem costs as well as animal care and use restrictions. Nevertheless, I propose here that there remain good prospects for homing pigeon research that could still profoundly influence how one understands aspects of avian navigation beyond sensory mechanisms and spatial-representational strategies. Indeed, research into neural mechanisms and brain organization, social/personality influences and genetics of navigation all offer opportunities to take advantage of the rich spatial behavior repertoire and experimental convenience of homing pigeons. Importantly, research in these areas would not necessarily require the large number of birds typically used in the past to study the sensory guidance of navigation. For those of us who have had the opportunity to work with this remarkable animal, one research door may be closing, but a window into exciting future opportunities lies ajar.

Introduction

Beginning in the 1950s, laboratories in the United Kingdom (Matthews, 1951) and Germany (Kramer, 1957) established the domesticated homing pigeon as the premier model for investigating the sensory basis and map-like properties of avian navigation. This continued up to the end of the 20th century while homing pigeon research flourished under the leadership of such notable scientists as William Keeton, Floriano Papi, Klaus Schmidt-Koenig, Charles Walcott, Hans Walraff, and Roswitha and Wolfgang Wiltschko, whose research teams and large-scale pigeon-loft operations produced an enormous volume of data and influential papers (see Wallraff, 2005; Gagliardo, 2013; Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2017a). However, only the Pisa lofts, now headed by Anna Gagliardo, continue in operation, and the pace of this rich history has substantially diminished because of a number of factors in addition to the vanishing of the large loft facilities. These factors include: (1) a growing consensus on the sensory and representational mechanisms underpinning pigeon navigation; (2) a technological expansion that enables a more complete analysis of the sensory and spatial (map)-representational mechanisms of navigation in long-distance migratory and other wild birds; and (3) prohibitive per diem costs imposed by universities as well as animal welfare regulations. Therefore, the question I raise here is whether the combination of a largely successful research program and other extrinsic factors has rendered the homing pigeon obsolete with respect to advancing our understanding of the sensory toolbox and spatial-representational mechanisms used by birds as a group to navigate. However, recognizing this does not mean the cupboard is bare, as there are still numerous other research challenges well suited for the use of homing pigeons as experimental subjects.

Are the sensory mechanisms of homing pigeon navigation known?

I would like to premise this section by admitting that a full understanding of any behavioral phenomenon is illusory, and is simply awaiting the next surprise. Having said that, a strong case can be made that, broadly speaking, the sensory mechanisms that guide homing pigeons after release from an unfamiliar location to the home loft are well understood, and that understanding is a testament to the researchers mentioned above, their students and collaborators. The intent here is not to thoroughly summarize the vast body of literature that enables the claim of understanding, but to offer a brief overview colored by my own interpretation of the accumulated knowledge regarding pigeons and homing. Detailed summaries and interpretations can be found in Wiltschko and Wiltschko (2003, 2017a), Wallraff (2005, 2014) and Gagliardo (2013).

It is in many ways remarkable that Kramer's (1957) ‘map and compass’ model of homing pigeon navigation and Walraff's (1974) ‘gradient’ model of how Kramer's map would be structured are as relevant today as when they were first introduced (Fig. 1). The map and compass model captures homing pigeon navigation as a two-step process in which pigeons, when released from a distant, unfamiliar location, first determine their position relative to the home loft with use of their ‘navigational map’, and then transform that relational-spatial information into a homeward-oriented compass bearing. The gradient model captures the representation of the map in the form of two intersecting stimulus gradients, which need not necessarily be perceived through the same sensory modality, that vary predictably in space. The crucial point is that an approximate understanding of the rate of stimulus change in a gradient through space, which can be implemented as a neural algorithm, would allow a pigeon to extrapolate its position relative to its home loft at distances beyond what they have experienced (unfamiliar locations) from as far away as the quality of the gradient change is spatially stable.

Glossary

Conditioning paradigm

A historically important, psychological experimental procedure by which the occurrence of a once neutral stimulus predicts the occurrence of another behaviorally meaningful stimulus presented at the same time or shortly thereafter. With repeated presentation of the paired stimuli, the previously neutral stimulus begins to elicit a behavioral response.

Functional lateralization

The observation that paired brain structures that reside in both brain hemispheres or in both sides of a nervous system, for example, the hippocampus on the left and right side of the cerebrum, have different physiological, emotional, behavioral or cognitive functions.

Hippocampal formation

Also identified as the dorsomedial pallium in non-mammalian vertebrates, this brain structure is fundamental for the control of map-like, spatial navigation and memory for events contextualized by space and time.

Homing speed

Generally, the average speed of an animal during its return from a start location to a goal location. For a homing pigeon, this would be the average speed during the flight from the release site until reaching the home loft.

Multi-modal map

A map used by an animal to guide navigation that is composed of more than one sensory element. For example, some have proposed that the navigational map of homing pigeons is supported by both olfactory and geomagnetic inputs.

Olfactory cortex

The region of the forebrain/telencephalon that receives direct inputs from the olfactory bulbs and participates in the neural processing of olfactory information.

Olfactory navigational map

The well-supported observation that the navigational map of homing pigeons, and other birds as well, used to return home is based on sufficiently stable variation in the spatial–environmental distribution of atmospheric odors.

Telencephalic organization

Refers to how the various subdivisions of the telencephalon are structured with respect to, among other things, neuronal cell types, connectivity among subdivisions (connectome) and neurochemistry.

Vanishing bearing

Before the time of GPS flight path recordings, the direction a homing pigeon would disappear from binocular view with respect to a release location.

Wingbeat signature

Typically characterized by the wingbeat frequency and flap–pause ratio of a flying animal.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Illustration of the complementarity of Kramer's (1957) ‘map and compass’ and Walraff's (1974) ‘gradient’ model of homing pigeon navigation. The map and compass model (bottom three panels) captures homing pigeon navigation as a two-step process in which a pigeon, when released from a distant, unfamiliar location, first determines its position relative to the home loft with use of its ‘navigational map’, and then transforms that relational-spatial information into a homeward-oriented compass bearing. The gradient model (top panel) captures the hypothetical representation of the map in the form of two intersecting stimulus gradients, in this example, two different odor profiles with intensities predictably varying along a northeast–southwest (odor profile A) and a northwest–southeast (odor profile B) axis. For advocates of a geomagnetic navigational map, the axes could be composed of variation in geomagnetic intensity or inclination. The crucial point is that an approximate understanding of the rate of stimulus change in a gradient, which can be learned through the experience of flying in the gray cloud of familiar space and implemented as a neural algorithm, would allow a pigeon to extrapolate its position relative to its home loft at distances beyond what they have experienced (unfamiliar locations outside of the gray cloud).

For decades, the challenge that dominated research into homing pigeon navigation was the sensory basis of the map and compass. It is now universally agreed that homing pigeons have two compass mechanisms: one reliant on the sun, the sun compass, and the other reliant on the Earth's magnetic field, the geomagnetic compass. Less agreed upon is the sensory basis(es) of the map and whether, like the compass cues, different maps can be constructed from different sensory inputs. Indeed, much of the controversy and public antagonism among researchers has been motivated by strongly held positions on the navigational map's sensory basis. However, what most researchers would now agree on is that atmospheric odors play a fundamental role in enabling homing pigeons to determine their location relative to home from unfamiliar locations (Wallraff, 2005, 2014; Gagliardo, 2013; but see Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2017b for a rebuttal of this position) – that is, the navigational map is olfactory in nature.

Less visible as a research topic is the recognized importance of familiar landmark and landscape features that pigeons exploit when they navigate closer to their home loft or over any previously experienced, familiar space. This ‘second map’ is of interest because it can be reliant on the avian hippocampus for its implementation (Herold et al., 2015; Bingman and MacDougall-Shackleton, 2017), and, when reliant on the hippocampus (see Gagliardo et al., 1999), its properties resemble those associated with the so-called cognitive map, as developed by O'Keefe and Nadel (1978).

Taken together, the brief outline above offers a credible and sufficient framework toward understanding the sensory guidance that pigeons use to navigate, a framework that is carefully and compellingly expounded by Wallraff (2005) and enables one to answer ‘yes’ to the question raised at the beginning of this section. When a pigeon is released from an unfamiliar location, it first relies on its olfactory navigational map (see Glossary) to approximate the direction of displacement with respect to the home loft. Wallraff (2005) also nicely describes the hypothetical properties of such an olfactory navigational map that addresses the intuitive obstacle of the disruptive effects of changing atmospheric conditions on the spatial reliability of chemical cues in the air. That information is then used to compute a homeward-oriented bearing directed by the sun or magnetic compass. As the pigeon gets closer to home and transitions from unfamiliar to familiar space, it begins to rely more on its hippocampal-dependent landmark/landscape map.

What is left to understand?

As hinted at above, many researchers in the field would be (perhaps justifiably) appalled by the simplistic sensory/representational guidance scheme of homing pigeon navigation outlined in the previous section. Certainly the mechanisms of homing pigeon navigation are much more nuanced than what I have summarized. As examples, questions remain about: the relationship between the sun and the magnetic compass; how might sun or magnetic-compass orientation interact with landmark-/landscape-derived spatial information; and does the navigational map necessarily need to rely on a gradient stimulus structure or can homing pigeons learn what is referred to as a ‘mosaic map’, in which positional information is extracted from spatially stable ‘patches’ of discrete sensory inputs? Given the adequacy of the navigational framework described above, which again is a simplified account of the much more carefully crafted thesis of Wallraff (2005), one can ask: have the big questions about the sensory guidance and spatial-representational structure of homing pigeon navigation been answered?

In my view, there are two conversations and one side note that are relevant with respect to this question. The first is the elusive and seemingly enduring question of whether something in addition to olfaction can support the homing pigeon navigational map. Here, one must acknowledge that there is tantalizing evidence that the Earth's magnetic field, in addition to supporting the magnetic compass, can also support a geomagnetic map (Dennis et al., 2007; Wiltschko et al., 2010; Schiffner et al., 2011; Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2017a). The evidence has prompted the idea of a navigational map that is multi-modal in nature (see Glossary; e.g. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2017a), which certainly has an intuitive appeal, and there is no reason why the multi-modality should be limited to olfaction and geomagnetism (see Hagstrum, 2000). Indeed, looking broadly at homing pigeon navigation, the system is necessarily multi-modal with, minimally, an olfactory navigational map operational from distant, unfamiliar sites and a principally visual familiar landmark/landscape map closer to home. Nonetheless, one is still left with the question that if olfaction sufficiently explains the navigational map of homing pigeons (Wallraff, 2005; Gagliardo, 2013), why introduce the idea of multi-modality in the absence of any compelling support other than its trendy appeal? It is axiomatic that one must be objective about the possibility of a map based on something in addition to olfaction, and, as noted above, there is evidence that experimental treatments designed to disrupt the extraction of positional information from the Earth's magnetic field (but some could also impact the geomagnetic compass) do influence the behavior of pigeons during homing. However, Anna Gagliardo routinely offers the sobering reminder that although magnetic treatments in homing pigeons have resulted in changes in behavior, those changes have never influenced the likelihood of pigeons returning home any slower than control-treated pigeons; in other words, there is little navigational impact of those treatments. Indeed, many magnetic treatments have no effect at all on homing behavior from unfamiliar locations (e.g. Holland et al., 2013), and one of the most used techniques designed to disrupt geomagnetic sensitivity in experiments investigating a magnetic map (e.g. Wiltschko et al., 2010), presumptive peripheral anesthesia of trigeminal nerve-coupled receptors, likely has no effect at all on geomagnetic sensitivity (Engels et al., 2018). Notable here is the contrast between the subtle effects of magnetic treatments on behavior compared with the complete failure of birds deprived of smell to navigate home successfully (Wallraff, 2005; Gagliardo, 2013). In my view, the most compelling evidence against anything other than an olfactory map in homing pigeons is the elegant study of Gagliardo et al. (2009). That study demonstrated that although cutting of the olfactory nerve in homing pigeons results, as expected, in a disruption of navigational ability, cuts of the trigeminal nerve – the presumptive source of peripheral geomagnetic-map inputs to the brain – have no impact on navigation. Finally, advocates of a geomagnetic map have not adequately addressed the challenge of how, over the distances they navigate (see Bingman and Cheng, 2005), homing pigeons would compensate for the temporal and spatial noise in the Earth's magnetic field.

From some perspectives, the GPS technology that developed in the 1990s allowed for an almost transformative opportunity to reveal much more about the navigation of homing pigeons by providing complete reconstructions of the routes pigeons take to return home. Such route reconstructions supply data that informationally go well beyond the earlier experimental recording of simple vanishing bearings (see Glossary) taken with binoculars and homing speeds (see Glossary). However, and perhaps surprisingly, the GPS tracking seems to have revealed much more about how pigeons move and navigate over familiar space (Lipp et al., 2004; Gagliardo et al., 2007; Guilford and Biro, 2014) than how sensory mechanisms control the navigational map used from unfamiliar locations. For example, using GPS tracking, Gagliardo et al. (2016) investigated olfactory map navigation, and Schiffner et al. (2011) studied geomagnetic map navigation, but the former study would not be considered instrumental in supporting an olfactory map and the latter offers no convincing evidence for a geomagnetic map. Given the research that is being conducted in migratory birds (see below), it is challenging to see how studying pigeon homing with sophisticated GPS tracking devices will unveil anything new about the sensory or map/representational mechanisms that support avian navigation in general. That said, there are other aspects of pigeon navigational behavior – for example, the study of the underlying neural mechanisms – that can be much more profoundly investigated with the use of GPS tracking (see below).

Finally, it must be noted that an often-discussed challenge to the olfactory navigation hypothesis has been developed by Jorge and colleagues (2009, 2010), who argue that olfactory stimulation is necessary to ‘activate’ the navigational map but positional information would be sourced to some other sensory input, e.g. geomagnetism. In other words, olfactory-deprived pigeons fail to home from unfamiliar locations not because atmospheric odors provide positional information, but simply because the navigational system was never ‘turned on’. However, as appealing as the ‘activational hypothesis’ may be for opponents of an olfactory navigational map, it is strongly contradicted by the available evidence (Gagliardo et al., 2001, 2011; Wallraff, 2014). Indeed, in a recent paper looking at thalamic activation under different homing treatments, Jorge et al. (2017), while still claiming an activational contribution from olfaction, also identify a direct role of olfaction in determining map-based position. Given, in my opinion, the overall untenable premise of the ‘activational hypothesis’, I continue to be amazed that it is arguably now the most debated issue regarding the sensory basis of the homing pigeon navigational map (see Walcott et al., 2018; Gagliardo et al., 2018).

The growth in understanding sensory guidance of navigation in migratory and other wild birds

The study of homing pigeons has been, in part, motivated by the assumption that, as an accessible experimental model, it could reveal much about how birds, and migratory birds in particular, generally navigate. However, the often staggering distances traveled by migratory birds, routinely thousands of kilometers, and their remarkable success in relocating back to their regular breeding sites both indicate that the navigational mechanisms employed, and their underlying sensory bases, are considerably more variable and collectively more complex than what is found in homing pigeons and their navigational system, which is operational over a few hundred kilometers (Bingman and Cheng, 2005). And indeed, with advances in tracking technologies and the data that will soon be available from the visionary ICARUS (International Cooperation for Animal Research Using Space) Initiative, headed by Martin Wikelski, investigating navigational mechanisms in wild birds is no longer limited by a lack of accessibility (see Guilford et al., 2011), and the ‘convenience’ of homing pigeons as an experimental model is not as appealing as it once was.

Tracking studies have already revealed tantalizing hints at the complex mechanisms that guide migratory navigation (e.g. Thorup et al., 2007), and some have even investigated supporting sensory mechanisms (Holland et al., 2009; Wikelski et al., 2015), which, perhaps surprisingly, hint at an important role of olfaction. Such field-based studies have been complemented by recent success investigating the sensory basis of migratory navigation using captive migrants tested in orientation cages. Whereas there is little compelling evidence for a geomagnetic map in homing pigeons navigating over relatively short distances (see above), for longer-distance migrants the evidence does point to at least partial positional information being derived from the Earth's magnetic field (Deutschlander et al., 2012; Kishkinev et al., 2015). However, given that these experiments were carried out in orientation cages, one has to wonder whether the results from captive birds would generalize to free-flying birds in the wild. For example, in nesting shearwaters navigating over distances of up to 800 km, there is little support for a geomagnetic map (see Pollonara et al., 2015; Wikelski et al., 2015). Nonetheless, the apparent contrast between homing pigeons and long-distance migratory birds, at least those tested in orientation cages, is consistent with hypothesized constraints on the plausibility of a geomagnetic map at shorter navigational distances because of the inevitable spatial and temporal errors in positional information derived from it (see Bingman and Cheng, 2005).

The advances in tracking technology are not limited to birds during migration, but can also be applied to the study of navigational and guiding sensory mechanisms used while moving between nest sites and remote feeding/displacement locations. For example, Pollonara et al. (2015) have demonstrated a role for olfaction in Scopoli's shearwaters (Calonectris diomedea), a Mediterranean seabird, navigating to nest sites, and Orchan et al. (2016) have investigated the development of navigational abilities in stone curlews (Burhinus oedicnemus), both facilitated by GPS tracking technology.

The claim that the navigational mechanisms used by experienced migratory birds, perhaps returning to a breeding site several thousand kilometers distant, are necessarily richer or more complex than those used by homing pigeons appears unassailable. I do realize, however, that ‘complex’ is an easy word to use without really defining it. In support of the reasoning that the navigational mechanisms of migrants are more complex, beyond the obvious factor of distance, is that migrants are more likely to experience weather conditions that will displace them from any familiar, previous route and they often need to detour around geographical barriers. In other words, and admittedly an oversimplification, once a homing pigeon takes up a homeward bearing, it is unlikely to need to change that bearing in any substantial way until arriving close to home. By contrast, a migrant being blown off course by a weather system or choosing to detour around, for example, the Gulf of Mexico, and all this happening perhaps several thousand kilometers away from the goal location, would require a continual updating and re-orientation from any one preferred migratory bearing. It should also be noted that different species and populations of migratory birds navigate under a variety of conditions, laying the foundation for comparative investigations into the factors determining which array of possible sensory and representational mechanisms are favored under different environmental conditions. Given that homing pigeon research can only partially capture the complexity and diversity associated with distance and the mechanisms guiding the continual directional adjustments migratory birds need to make, the recent experimental accessibility of studying navigational mechanisms in migrating and other wild birds conspires to diminish the value of homing pigeons as a prototype or model species.

A glass half full

Although homing pigeons may no longer be as useful as a general model for the sensory mechanisms that guide avian navigation, there are nonetheless other aspects of avian spatial behavior where homing pigeons can be of enormous experimental value. The homing pigeon brain continues to be the avian brain in which overall telencephalic organization (see Glossary) is best understood (Shanahan et al., 2013), and therefore homing pigeons are still best suited for studies investigating the underlying neural mechanisms that guide navigation and doing so in a field setting. For example, homing pigeons have been crucial in understanding the importance of the avian hippocampal formation (see Glossary; Herold et al., 2015; Bingman and MacDougall-Shackleton, 2017) and olfactory cortex (see Glossary; Patzke et al., 2010) for navigation. The avian brain is characterized by a strong degree of structural and functional lateralization (see Glossary; Manns and Ströckens, 2014), and many avian navigational mechanisms are similarly lateralized and best understood in homing pigeons (Bingman and Gagliardo, 2006; Gagliardo et al., 2011; Jonckers et al., 2015), including the lateralization of visual guidance during homing (Martinho et al., 2015; Pollonara et al., 2017). And even though we know of the sensory mechanisms that guide avian navigation, the underlying neural control of those mechanisms is still poorly understood [but see the important findings of Zapka et al. (2009) revealing the importance of the anterior forebrain ‘Cluster N’ in supporting the geomagnetic compass of migratory birds]. Using homing pigeons, Cordula Mora and colleagues (Mora and Bingman, 2013; Mora et al., 2014) have developed an operant conditioning paradigm (see Glossary) that can be used to examine the perceptual range of magnetic field detection and may lead to an understanding of the properties and limits of geomagnetic intensity and inclination sensitivity. However, and perhaps of more importance, the same paradigm could be used to identify the network of brain regions that support the geomagnetic compass, and, should homing pigeons ever be shown to possess a geomagnetic map (but see above), the brain regions that implement a geomagnetic map too.

GPS tracking has also been recently used to identify differences in wingbeat characteristics, which were reliably associated with differences in acceleration, body amplitude and airspeed when pigeons fly a familiar route (Taylor et al., 2017). From a mechanistic perspective, wingbeat signatures (see Glossary) could also be used as a proxy for identifying when homing pigeons switch between navigational and sensory mechanisms being employed and, by inference, the neural mechanisms engaged. A similar approach has been developed by Schiffner et al. (2011), who used behavioral indicators, but not wingbeat characteristics, to mathematically model inferences of shifts in navigational mechanism(s) active during a homing flight. Although admittedly less informative, recording changes in wingbeat patterns would also be much easier to use than recording directly from the brain (Vyssotski et al., 2009) to identify when pigeons may be making changes in the navigational mechanism(s) currently controlling behavior, but only as long as any changes in wingbeat characteristics owing to non-navigational factors, e.g. shifts in wind profile, can be excluded. The important point is that such detailed behavioral-modeling approaches for understanding dynamic changes in navigational strategy or mechanism are currently only feasible in homing pigeons (but see above) because of their size, which enables the carrying of both GPS recorders and accelerometers, and their faithfulness in returning home, which allows for the recovery of data that cannot be transmitted remotely.

The tracking of homing pigeons has also proved illuminating with respect to insights into how idiosyncratic spatial behavioral profiles of individuals, ‘spatial personalities’ and social interactions impact navigation (Nagy et al., 2010; Freeman et al., 2011; Portugal et al., 2017). This line of research is of considerable relevance to the growing literature on animal personality (e.g. Sih et al., 2012). ‘Social navigation’ studied in homing pigeons would likely offer important insights into the navigational consequences of group migrations, and application of this pioneering work in homing pigeons has already been used to study group migration in white storks (Ciconia ciconia; Flack et al., 2018). Homing pigeons and related forms of domesticated rock doves are well suited for uncovering correlations between genetic variation and spatial behavior characteristics (Domyan and Shapiro, 2017; Gazda et al., 2018), which will be facilitated by the complete genome sequencing of individual homing pigeons and members of other breeds of pigeon. Gazing to the future, the application of CRISPR-based genome editing and the creation of transgenic pigeons could offer a vast, new frontier for understanding the genetics of navigation and associated neural support mechanisms, as well as perhaps the engineering of a super-navigator. Although genetic experiments would likely require large numbers of birds and resources (perhaps justifying the re-establishment of new, large-scale loft facilities), homing pigeons, because of their experimental accessibility, are likely much better suited than any other candidate migratory bird model for investigating the genetics of navigation.

Concluding remarks

It may seem unusual that someone on the margins of research into the sensory mechanisms of homing pigeon navigation should suggest that the big questions appear to be answered and the usefulness of homing pigeons for generally understanding the mechanisms of long-distance navigation in birds as a group may be ending. Indeed, those still actively working with homing pigeons would likely challenge this view. However, the sobering prognosis offered here is rendered more real by the almost complete loss of the historically dominant, industrial-scale pigeon-loft facilities and, perhaps more importantly, the burgeoning of new tracking technologies making navigational experiments on long-distance migratory and other birds much more accessible.

At the same time, the impact of homing pigeon research may still profoundly influence how researchers can understand aspects of avian navigation beyond sensory mechanisms and spatial-representational strategies – for example, the gradient properties of the navigational map or the landscape/landmark properties of a familiar-area map. Research into neural mechanisms and brain organization, social/personality influences and genetics of navigation all offer opportunities to exploit the rich spatial behavior repertoire and experimental convenience of homing pigeons. Importantly, research in these areas would not necessarily require the large number of birds historically used to study the sensory guidance of navigation, although for some imaginable genetic investigations, large numbers of animals would likely be needed. For those of us who have been blessed with the opportunity to work with this remarkable animal, one research door may be closing, but other exciting opportunities beckon.

Acknowledgements

I offer sincere and heartfelt thanks to Ken Able, Paola Bagnoli, Anna Gagliardo, Paolo Ioalé, the late Floriano Papi, and Wolfgang and Roswitha Wiltschko, all of whom in some way gave me the opportunity to appreciate and research the amazing homing pigeon. I am grateful to David Gesicki and Karissa Payne, who offered useful comments for improving the manuscript. Karissa also prepared Fig. 1. David Hatton, as developmental editor, and Anna Gagliardo and David Keays, as reviewers, also offered numerous suggestions for improving the manuscript, and I am sincerely grateful for their feedback.

FOOTNOTES

  • Competing interests

    The author declares no competing or financial interests.

  • Funding

    The Commentary was written while V.P.B. was supported by National Science Foundation grant IOS-1457304 (Directorate for Biological Sciences).

  • © 2018. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bingman, V. P. and
    2. Cheng, K.
    (2005). Mechanisms of animal global navigation: comparative perspectives and enduring challenges. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 17, 295-318. doi:10.1080/08927014.2005.9522584
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    1. Bingman, V. P. and
    2. Gagliardo, A.
    (2006). Of birds and men: convergent evolution in hippocampal lateralization and spatial cognition. Cortex 42, 99-100. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70329-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  3. ↵
    1. Bingman, V. P. and
    2. MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A.
    (2017). The avian hippocampus and the hypothetical maps used by navigating migratory birds (with some reflection on compasses and migratory restlessness). J. Comp. Physiol. A. 203, 465-474. doi:10.1007/s00359-017-1161-0
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Dennis, T. E.,
    2. Rayner, M. J. and
    3. Walker, M. M.
    (2007). Evidence that pigeons orient to geomagnetic intensity during homing. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 274, 1153-1158. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.3768
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Deutschlander, M. E.,
    2. Phillips, J. B. and
    3. Munro, U.
    (2012). Age-dependent orientation to magnetically-simulated geographic displacements in migratory Australian silvereyes (Zosterops l. lateralis). Wilson J. Ornithol. 124, 467-477. doi:10.1676/11-043.1
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    1. Domyan, E. T. and
    2. Shapiro, M. D.
    (2017). Pigeonetics takes flight: evolution, development, and genetics of intraspecific variation. Dev. Biol. 427, 241-250. doi:10.1016/j.ydbio.2016.11.008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Engels, S.,
    2. Treiber, C. D.,
    3. Salzer, M. C.,
    4. Michalik, A.,
    5. Ushakova, L.,
    6. Keays, D. A.,
    7. Mouritsen, H. and
    8. Heyers, D.
    (2018). Lidocaine is a nocebo treatment for trigeminally mediated magnetic orientation in birds. J. R. Soc. Interface 15, 20180124. doi:10.1098/rsif.2018.0124
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Flack, A.,
    2. Nagy, M.,
    3. Fiedler, W.,
    4. Couzin, I. D. and
    5. Wikelski, M.
    (2018). From local collective behavior to global migratory patterns in white storks. Science 360, 911-914. doi:10.1126/science.aap7781
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Freeman, R.,
    2. Mann, R.,
    3. Guilford, T. and
    4. Biro, D.
    (2011). Group decisions and individual differences: route fidelity predicts flight leadership in homing pigeons (Columba livia). Biol. letters. 7, 63-66. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2010.0627
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.
    (2013). Forty years of olfactory navigation in birds. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 2165-2171. doi:10.1242/jeb.070250
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Ioalé, P. and
    3. Bingman, V. P.
    (1999). Homing in pigeons: the role of the hippocampal formation in the representation of landmarks used for navigation. J. Neurosci. 19, 311-315. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00311.1999
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Odetti, F. and
    3. Ioalè, P.
    (2001). Relevance of visual cues for orientation at familiar sites by homing pigeons: an experiment in a circular arena. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 268, 2065-2070. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1746
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Ioalè, P.,
    3. Savini, M.,
    4. Lipp, H.-P. and
    5. Dell'Omo, G.
    (2007). Finding home: the final step of the pigeons’ homing process studied with a GPS data logger. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 1132-1138. doi:10.1242/jeb.003244
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Ioalè, P.,
    3. Savini, M. and
    4. Wild, M.
    (2009). Navigational abilities of adult and experienced homing pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic information. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 3119-3124. doi:10.1242/jeb.031864
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Filannino, C.,
    3. Ioalè, P.,
    4. Pecchia, T.,
    5. Wikelski, M. and
    6. Vallortigara, G.
    (2011). Olfactory lateralization in homing pigeons: a GPS study on birds released with unilateral olfactory inputs. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 593-598. doi:10.1242/jeb.049510
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Pollonara, E. and
    3. Wikelski, M.
    (2016). Pigeon navigation: exposure to environmental odours prior to release is sufficient for homeward orientation, but not for homing. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 2475-2480. doi:10.1242/jeb.140889
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Gagliardo, A.,
    2. Pollonara, E. and
    3. Wikelski, M.
    (2018). Only natural local odors allow homeward orientation in homing pigeons released at unfamiliar sites. J. Comp. Physiol. A 204, 761-771. doi:10.1007/s00359-018-1277-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Gazda, M. A.,
    2. Andrade, P.,
    3. Afonso, S.,
    4. Dilyte, J.,
    5. Archer, J. P.,
    6. Lopes, R. J.,
    7. Faria, R. and
    8. Carneiro, M.
    (2018). Signatures of selection on standing genetic variation underlie athletic and navigational performance in racing pigeons. Mol. Biol. Evol. 35, 1176-1189. doi:10.1093/molbev/msy030
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Guilford, T. and
    2. Biro, D.
    (2014). Route following and the pigeon's familiar area map. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 169-179. doi:10.1242/jeb.092908
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Guilford, T.,
    2. Åkesson, S.,
    3. Gagliardo, A.,
    4. Holland, R. A.,
    5. Mouritsen, H.,
    6. Muheim, R.,
    7. Wiltschko, R.,
    8. Wiltschko, W. and
    9. Bingman, V. P.
    (2011). Migratory navigation in birds: new opportunities in an era of fast-developing tracking technology. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 3705-3712. doi:10.1242/jeb.051292
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Holland, R.,
    2. Filannino, C. and
    3. Gagliardo, A.
    (2013). A magnetic pulse does not affect homing pigeon navigation: a GPS tracking experiment. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 2192-2200. doi:10.1242/jeb.083543
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Hagstrum, J. T.
    (2000). Infrasound and the avian navigational map. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 1103-1111.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  23. ↵
    1. Herold, C.,
    2. Coppola, V. J. and
    3. Bingman, V. P.
    (2015). The maturation of research into the avian hippocampal formation: recent discoveries from one of the nature's foremost navigators. Hippocampus 25, 1193-1211. doi:10.1002/hipo.22463
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Holland, R. A.,
    2. Thorup, K.,
    3. Gagliardo, A.,
    4. Bisson, I. A.,
    5. Knecht, E.,
    6. Mizrahi, D. and
    7. Wikelski, M.
    (2009). Testing the role of sensory systems in the migratory heading of a songbird. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 4065-4071. doi:10.1242/jeb.034504
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Jonckers, E.,
    2. Güntürkün, O.,
    3. De Groof, G.,
    4. Van der Linden, A. and
    5. Bingman, V. P.
    (2015). Network structure of functional hippocampal lateralization in birds. Hippocampus 25, 1418-1428. doi:10.1002/hipo.22462
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. ↵
    1. Jorge, P. E.,
    2. Marques, A. E. and
    3. Phillips, J. B.
    (2009). Activational rather than navigational effects of odors on homing of young pigeons. Curr. Biol. 19, 650-654. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.066
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. ↵
    1. Jorge, P. E.,
    2. Marques, P. A. M. and
    3. Phillips, J. B.
    (2010). Activational effects of odours on avian navigation. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 277, 45-49. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1521
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Jorge, P. E.,
    2. Pinto, B. V.,
    3. Bingman, V. P. and
    4. Phillips, J. B.
    (2017). Involvement of the avian dorsal thalamic nuclei in homing pigeon navigation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 213. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00213
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Kishkinev, D.,
    2. Chernetsov, N.,
    3. Pakhomov, A.,
    4. Heyers, D. and
    5. Mouritsen, H.
    (2015). Eurasian reed warblers compensate for virtual magnetic displacement. Curr. Biol. 25, R822-R824. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Kramer, G.
    (1957). Experiments on bird orientation and their interpretation. Ibis 99, 196-227. doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1957.tb01947.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    1. Lipp, H.-P.,
    2. Vyssotski, A. L.,
    3. Wolfer, D. P.,
    4. Renaudineau, S.,
    5. Savini, M.,
    6. Tröster, G. and
    7. Dell'Omo, G.
    (2004). Pigeon homing along highways and exits. Curr. Biol. 14, 1239-1249. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.024
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. ↵
    1. Manns, M. and
    2. Ströckens, F.
    (2014). Functional and structural comparison of visual lateralization in birds–similar but still different. Front. Psychol. 5, 206. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Martinho, A., III.,
    2. Biro, D.,
    3. Guilford, T.,
    4. Gagliardo, A. and
    5. Kacelnik, A.
    (2015). Asymmetric visual input and route recapitulation in homing pigeons. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151957. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1957
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Matthews, G. T.
    (1951). The experimental investigation of navigation in homing pigeons. J. Exp. Biol. 28, 508-536.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  35. ↵
    1. Mora, C. V. and
    2. Bingman, V. P.
    (2013). Detection of magnetic field intensity gradient by homing pigeons (Columba livia) in a novel ‘virtual magnetic map’ conditioning paradigm. PLoS ONE 8, 1-17. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072869
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Mora, C. V.,
    2. Acerbi, M. L. and
    3. Bingman, V. P.
    (2014). Conditioned discrimination of magnetic inclination in a spatial-orientation arena task by homing pigeons (Columba livia). J. Exp. Biol. 217, 4123-4131. doi:10.1242/jeb.101113
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Nagy, M.,
    2. Ákos, Z.,
    3. Biro, D. and
    4. Vicsek, T.
    (2010). Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature 464, 890-893. doi:10.1038/nature08891
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  38. ↵
    1. O'Keefe, J. and
    2. Nadel, L.
    (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  39. ↵
    1. Orchan, Y.,
    2. Ovaskainen, O.,
    3. Bouten, W. and
    4. Nathan, R.
    (2016). Novel insights into the map stage of true navigation in nonmigratory wild birds (stone curlews, Burhinus oedicnemus). Am. Nat. 187, 152-165. doi:10.1086/686054
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    1. Patzke, N.,
    2. Manns, M.,
    3. Güntürkün, O.,
    4. Ioalè, P. and
    5. Gagliardo, A.
    (2010). Navigation-induced ZENK expression in the olfactory system of pigeons (Columba livia). Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 2062-2072. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07240.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Pollonara, E.,
    2. Luschi, P.,
    3. Guilford, T.,
    4. Wikelski, M.,
    5. Bonadonna, F. and
    6. Gagliardo, A.
    (2015). Olfaction and topography, but not magnetic cues, control navigation in a pelagic seabird: displacements with shearwaters in the Mediterranean Sea. Sci. Rep. 5, 16486. doi:10.1038/srep16486
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Pollonara, E.,
    2. Guilford, T.,
    3. Rossi, M.,
    4. Bingman, V. P. and
    5. Gagliardo, A.
    (2017). Right hemisphere advantage in the development of route fidelity in homing pigeons. Anim. Behav. 123, 395-409. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.11.019
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    1. Portugal, S. J.,
    2. Ricketts, R. L.,
    3. Chappell, J.,
    4. White, C. R.,
    5. Shepard, E. L. and
    6. Biro, D.
    (2017). Boldness traits, not dominance, predict exploratory flight range and homing behaviour in homing pigeons. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 372, 20160234. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0234
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Schiffner, I.,
    2. Fuhrmann, P. and
    3. Wiltschko, R.
    (2011). Tracking pigeons in a magnetic anomaly and in magnetically “quiet” terrain. Naturwissenschaften 98, 575-581. doi:10.1007/s00114-011-0802-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  45. ↵
    1. Shanahan, M.,
    2. Bingman, V. P.,
    3. Shimizu, T.,
    4. Wild, M. and
    5. Güntürkün, O.
    (2013). Large-scale network organization in the avian forebrain: a connectivity matrix and theoretical analysis. Front. Comput. Neurosc. 7, 89. doi:10.3389/fncom.2013.00089
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Sih, A.,
    2. Cote, J.,
    3. Evans, M.,
    4. Fogarty, S. and
    5. Pruitt, J.
    (2012). Ecological implications of behavioural syndromes. Ecol. Lett. 15, 278-289. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01731.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Taylor, L. A.,
    2. Portugal, S. J. and
    3. Biro, D.
    (2017). Homing pigeons (Columba livia) modulate wingbeat characteristics as a function of route familiarity. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 2908-2915. doi:10.1242/jeb.154039
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Thorup, K.,
    2. Bisson, I.-A.,
    3. Bowlin, M. S.,
    4. Holland, R. A.,
    5. Wingfield, J. C.,
    6. Ramenofsky, M. and
    7. Wikelski, M.
    (2007). Evidence for a navigational map stretching across the continental US in a migratory songbird. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 18115-18119. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704734104
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Vyssotski, A. L.,
    2. Dell'Omo, G.,
    3. Dell'Ariccia, G.,
    4. Abramchuk, A. N.,
    5. Serkov, A. N.,
    6. Latanov, A. V.,
    7. Loizzo, A.,
    8. Wolfer, D. P. and
    9. Lipp, H.-P.
    (2009). EEG responses to visual landmarks in flying pigeons. Curr. Biol. 19, 1159-1166. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.070
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. ↵
    1. Walcott, C.,
    2. Wiltschko, W.,
    3. Wiltschko, R. and
    4. Zupanc, G. K. H.
    (2018). Olfactory navigation versus olfactory activation: a controversy revisited. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202, 703. doi:10.1007/s00359-018-1273-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    1. Wallraff, H. G.
    (1974). Das Navigationssystem der Vögel. Munich: Oldenbourg Verlag.
  52. ↵
    1. Wallraff, H. G.
    (2005). Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm. Berlin: Springer Verlag.
  53. ↵
    1. Wallraff, H. G.
    (2014). Do olfactory stimuli provide positional information for home-oriented avian navigation? Anim. Behav. 90, 1-6. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.012
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  54. ↵
    1. Wikelski, M.,
    2. Arriero, E.,
    3. Gagliardo, A.,
    4. Holland, R. A.,
    5. Huttunen, M. J.,
    6. Juvaste, R.,
    7. Mueller, I.,
    8. Tertitski, G.,
    9. Thorup, K.,
    10. Wild, M. et al.
    (2015). True navigation in migrating gulls requires intact olfactory nerves. Sci. Rep. 5, 17061. doi:10.1038/srep17061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Wiltschko, R.
    2. Wiltschko, W.
    (2003). Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts. Anim. Behav. 65, 257-272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  56. ↵
    1. Wiltschko, W. and
    2. Wiltschko, R.
    (2017a). Homing pigeons as a model for avian navigation? J. Avian Biol. 48, 66-74. doi:10.1111/jav.01270
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. ↵
    1. Wiltschko, R. and
    2. Wiltschko, W.
    (2017b). Considerations on the role of olfactory input in avian navigation. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 4347-4350. doi:10.1242/jeb.168302
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Wiltschko, R.,
    2. Schiffner, I.,
    3. Fuhrmann, P. and
    4. Wiltschko, W.
    (2010). The role of the magnetite-based receptors in the beak in pigeon homing. Curr. Biol., 20, 1534-1538. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Zapka, M.,
    2. Heyers, D.,
    3. Hein, C. M.,
    4. Engles, S.,
    5. Schneider, N.-L.,
    6. Hans, J.,
    7. Weiler, S.,
    8. Dreyer, D.,
    9. Kishkivev, D.,
    10. Wild, J. et al.
    (2009). Visual but not trigeminal mediation of magnetic compass information in a migratory bird. Nature 461, 1274-1277. doi:10.1038/nature08528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • Navigational map
  • Familiar area map
  • Migratory navigation
  • Olfaction
  • Geomagnetism
  • Bird

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Requiem for a heavyweight – can anything more be learned from homing pigeons about the sensory and spatial-representational basis of avian navigation?
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
COMMENTARY
Requiem for a heavyweight – can anything more be learned from homing pigeons about the sensory and spatial-representational basis of avian navigation?
Verner P. Bingman
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb163089 doi: 10.1242/jeb.163089 Published 22 October 2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
COMMENTARY
Requiem for a heavyweight – can anything more be learned from homing pigeons about the sensory and spatial-representational basis of avian navigation?
Verner P. Bingman
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb163089 doi: 10.1242/jeb.163089 Published 22 October 2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • Introduction
    • Are the sensory mechanisms of homing pigeon navigation known?
    • What is left to understand?
    • The growth in understanding sensory guidance of navigation in migratory and other wild birds
    • A glass half full
    • Concluding remarks
    • Acknowledgements
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Thermal robustness of biomechanical processes
  • Help, there are ‘omics’ in my comparative physiology!
  • Structural plasticity of the avian pectoralis: a case for geometry and the forgotten organelle
Show more COMMENTARY

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


The Big Biology podcast

JEB author Christine Cooper talks to Big Biology about her research. In this fascinating JEB sponsored podcast she tells us how tough zebra finches adjust their physiology to cope with extreme climate events. 


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992