Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) use visual contrast in self-assessment of camouflage
David Wilby, Samuel Riches, Ilse M. Daly, Andrew Bird, Matthew Wheelwright, James J. Foster
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb173831 doi: 10.1242/jeb.173831 Published 4 July 2018
David Wilby
1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for David Wilby
  • For correspondence: drdavidwilby@gmail.com
Samuel Riches
1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ilse M. Daly
1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ilse M. Daly
Andrew Bird
1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Matthew Wheelwright
1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
James J. Foster
2Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for James J. Foster
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Animals can make use of camouflage to reduce the likelihood of visual detection or recognition and thus improve their chances of survival. Background matching, where body colouration is closely matched to the surrounding substrate, is one form of camouflage. Hermit crabs have the opportunity to choose their camouflage independently of body colouration as they inhabit empty gastropod shells, making them ideal to study their choice of camouflage. We used 3D-printed artificial shells of varying contrasts against a grey substrate to test whether hermit crabs prefer shells that they perceive as less conspicuous. Contrast-minimising shells were chosen for Weber contrasts stronger than −0.5. However, in looming experiments, animals responded to contrasts as weak as −0.2, indicating that while they can detect differences between shells and the background, they are only motivated to move into those shells when the alternatives contrast strongly. This suggests a trade-off between camouflage and vulnerability introduced by switching shells.

INTRODUCTION

Background-matching camouflage is a common trait amongst animals; this form of cryptic body colouration has the goal of reducing the likelihood of an animal's visual detection by predators. While the majority of species are constrained by their external body patterning, there are many that have evolved the ability to dynamically tune these patterns, such as cephalopods, chameleons and some fish (e.g. Hanlon, 2007; Stuart-Fox et al., 2006; Smithers et al., 2017). However, colouration on its own is insufficient to avoid detection and cryptically coloured animals must also position themselves in the environment that they are best adapted to match (e.g. Kettlewell and Conn, 1977; Gillis, 1982; Tyrie et al., 2014; Marshall et al., 2016).

One group of animals with an unusual opportunity for behavioural crypsis is the hermit crabs, which inhabit empty gastropod shells to protect their soft abdomens and to provide a mobile shelter. As these shells almost completely cover their bodies (apart from the eyes and part of the legs), hermit crabs have the potential to change their colouration via choice of shell over relatively short periods of time; although, as these crabs are thought to possess only a single spectral class of photoreceptor (Stieve, 1960; Cronin and Forward, 1988), changes in their own colouration perceptible to these species are probably limited to shell shade and patterning. The behavioural aspects of shell choice have been well studied and various shell properties influence the choice of shell, including the weight, density, size, scent and shade (Lancaster, 1988, and references therein). Self-selection of camouflage is typically observed as the choice of appropriate backgrounds by a freely moving animal: behavioural crypsis. This choice is generally presumed to be driven by the animal's intuitions about the match between its own body colouration and the background (e.g. Kettlewell and Conn, 1977; Gillis, 1982; Merilaita and Jormalainen, 1997; Marshall et al., 2016), provided other factors, such as the thermal properties of the background (Ahnesjö and Forsman, 2006), are controlled for. In most cases, direct control of the degree of similarity between an animal and its background is not possible, as natural colouration varies even within phenotypes, and experimental alteration of an animal's colouration does not necessarily alter its instinctive camouflaging behaviour (e.g. Gillis, 1982).

Previous experiments have shown that hermit crabs select shells and backgrounds dependent on their contrast (as subjectively estimated by the experimenters). Reese (1963) allowed Pagurus samuelis without shells to approach ‘dark’ or ‘light’ shells situated within glass containers so as to isolate them in terms of tactile and olfactory cues, but leaving them visible. In this case, the animals preferred those shells of the opposite shade to the background. Partridge (1980) conducted experiments with Pagurus hirsutiusculus occupying shells painted ‘black’ or ‘white’, and showed that black shells are preferred (though the author makes no note of the substrate in this case) and that substrates of similar shade to an occupied shell are preferred. Most recently, Briffa et al. (2008) showed that common hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) select black shells on a black substrate and yellow shells on a yellow substrate, this behaviour being suppressed in the simulated presence of a predator. Gravel et al. (2004) also demonstrated that long-clawed hermit crabs (Pagurus longicarpus) would accept artificial shells produced via stereolithography in the absence of genuine mollusc shells.

Clearly, it is a common strategy among hermit crabs to choose shells that minimise their visibility against the background but there is currently no understanding of how this behaviour is mediated by vision. In this investigation, we sought to develop an understanding of how this camouflaging behaviour is adjusted depending on an animal's perceived contrast. We used models of P. bernhardus vision to calculate the perceived contrast of targets used in two types of behavioural experiment. In the first, hermit crabs chose between shells of various contrasts to the background and in the second, looming startle stimuli were presented on a computer monitor to examine hermit crabs' perception of contrast during a different visual task. For our behavioural crypsis experiment, we used 3D printed artificial shells, all of which were constructed using the same design and from the same material. In combination with the roughened polythene base that made up the substrate in these experiments, these artificial shells permitted control over the visual similarity between the animal (when inhabiting a shell) and its background, as well as over potentially confounding factors such as shell shape, density and odour (Mesce, 1982).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Animals [Pagurus bernhardus (Linnaeus 1758)] were collected from rockpools at Hannafore Point, Looe, Cornwall, UK. Individuals were identified and sexed by morphological criteria (Lancaster, 1988). Only males were used in this study as gravid females demonstrate a reluctance to switch shells (Briffa et al., 2008). Animals were housed in artificial seawater (TropicMarin Salt, Wartenberg, Germany) maintained at 18°C and 35–40ppt salinity. Lighting was by broadband white LED (PowerLED, Aldermaston, UK) (Fig. 1D) on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Methodological aspects. (A) Mathematical model of the shell used to design the 3D printed artificial shells. (B) Hermit crabs were able to inhabit artificial shells – seen here outside the experimental setup in a photographic tank. (C) Setup for the shell choice experiments. (D) Relative sensitivity of the 505 nm peak visual pigment, and relative quantal irradiance of LEDs in the experimental arena. (E) Reflectance spectra of the background-matching (curve 0) and ­background-contrasting shells (1–5). (F) Setup for the looming experiments. CRT, cathode ray tube.

3D-printed artificial shells

Artificial shells were designed using parametric equations (Cortie, 1993) in Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) (Fig. 1A). Digital 3D designs were prepared in Blender (Blender Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands) and MakerBot Desktop (Stratasys, Eden Prairie, MN, USA). Shell models were printed in white polylactic acid (PLA; RS Components, Corby, UK) using PVA filament (TechnologyOutlet, Leicester, UK) as a support material. The 3D printer was a Flashforge Creator Pro dual-material printer (Flashforge Corp., Zhejiang, China).

Support material was dissolved away in water and shells were spray painted with primer (Fine surface primer, white; Tamiya America Inc., Irvine, CA, USA), two layers of spray paint (Tamiya) and two layers of transparent matt spray varnish (acrylic, anti-shine matt varnish; The Army Painter, Hoerning, Denmark) (Fig. 1B).

Flat tiles of the same material were also painted in the same manner. All painted materials were soaked in salt water for 1 week and rinsed in freshwater. The reflectance spectra of the paints were measured from tiles using a spectrophotometer (QE65000, Ocean Optics Inc., Dunedin, FL, USA), a UV-Vis light source (DH-2000 Ocean Optics) and reflectance fibre probe (Ocean Optics) (Fig. 1E).

Shell choice experiments

Shell choice experiments took place inside 12 cm diameter circular polythene containers with roughened bases (Fig. 1C), painted in the same manner as the shells with paint shade number 0 (Fig. 1E) and filled to a depth of 7 cm with seawater.

Animals were removed from their natural shells prior to the experiment using a probe made of fishing line, which was inserted through a hole drilled near the apex of the shell. They were placed individually, without a shell, into the experimental arena inside a vertical opaque cylindrical tube at the centre of the arena to separate them from the shells during a 1 h rest phase before the start of the experiment. Tests were performed six at a time with each crab's container situated within a larger, diffusely illuminated isolated arena. Animals were allowed to choose between four artificial shells: two of the same shade as the background (paint shade 0; Fig. 1E) and two of a darker, contrasting shade (paint shades 1–5; Fig. 1E).

Following removal of the barriers, animals were left undisturbed for 6 h. During this time, the animals' behaviour was monitored via a webcam above each container, programmed to record one image per minute. The first and last choices of shell were recorded from the image sequences.

Six animals were tested on each day and each animal only saw each treatment once. The order of animals and which container position they were tested in was randomised throughout the set of experiments. A total of between 10 and 12 animals were tested in each treatment.

Biases for choices of background-matching over non-background-matching shells were determined via one-tailed binomial tests for a greater than 50% probability of choosing matching shells. Statistical tests were performed in R 3.1.0 (www.R-project.org/) implemented in R Studio 1.0.136 (http://www.rstudio.com/).

Looming experiments

To put the shell choice experiments within the wider context of achromatic vision in hermit crabs, a second set of behavioural experiments was performed using ‘looming disk’ startle stimuli, similar to previous work using this stimulus with marine crustaceans including hermit crabs (e.g. Tomsic, 2016; Shragai et al., 2017). Animals were held using a clamp in a transparent acrylic tank in front of a cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor (S96D, Videoseven, Ingram, Machotron Gmbh, Dornach, Germany) (Fig. 1F). The stimulus background was a consistent grey value throughout each session of presentations. Looming stimuli consisted of an expanding dark disc in the centre of the display which subtended a visual angle of 40 deg at their maximum. Preliminary tests showed that startle responses to these stimuli were robust but were prone to habituation following around 10 very high contrast stimuli (Weber contrast approaching −1); therefore, the number of presentations per session was kept below this number. The grey value of the loom was varied randomly between presentations, including a control stimulus of the same value as the background, by definition giving zero contrast. Animals were habituated for 10 min before the start of the presentations and the time between presentations varied randomly between 2 and 3 min. Two different treatments were presented; a brighter and a darker background, with each session consisting of stimuli from only one treatment. A total of 11 animals were used and each animal was tested in two treatments and saw every stimulus. Stimulus contrasts were calculated relative to each background treatment. The experimenter was visually isolated from the experiment by an opaque screen. Animals were observed and recorded using a video camera and monitor external to the experimental setup. Videos were graded as either no response (0) or response (1); responses included: a stop in normal behaviour or a flick of the antennae, partial retraction into the shell or full retraction.

Results were analysed by a one-tailed binomial test of the proportion of animals that responded against the response proportion at zero contrast. Statistical tests were performed in R 3.1.0 (http://www.R-project.org/) implemented in R Studio 1.0.136 (http://www.rstudio.com/).

Contrast calculation

Visual contrasts were calculated using relative quantum catch values modelled from measured irradiance and reflection spectra (Fig. 1D,F). In the shell choice experiment, the relative quantum catch of the single spectral class of photoreceptor in the hermit crab (Stieve, 1960; Cronin and Forward, 1988) was calculated for each shade (i=0–5) as:Embedded Image (1)where Ri is the reflection spectrum of shade i (Fig. 1E); S(λ) is the normalised visual pigment absorbance curve according to an A1 visual pigment template (Fig. 1D) (Govardovskii et al., 2000) peaking at 505 nm (Stieve, 1960); and I(λ) is the normalised quantal irradiance spectrum of the illuminating LED light source (Fig. 1D) (Kelber et al., 2003).

Weber contrast, Ci, was calculated as:Embedded Image (2)where Q0 is the quantum catch of the background. Contrast was calculated analogously in startle experiments, except with relative quantum catch defined as:Embedded Image (3)where Ii(λ) is the absolute irradiance spectrum of the monitor at the corresponding greyscale value; Q0 (in Eqn 2) is the quantum catch of the background and Qi is the quantum catch of the looming stimulus.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Animals readily occupied the artificial shells and over the course of each experiment moved between the available shells, similar to behaviour observed under natural conditions. For the three lowest contrast levels, the proportions of initial or final choices of shell were not significantly different from the chance level of 0.5 (Fig. 2A). For contrasts stronger than −0.45, more crabs chose matching than non-matching shells as both their initial and final choices, but only for the final choices was a significantly (Weber contrast=−0.4735; binomial test, P=0.01074, n=10) or marginally significantly (Weber contrast=−0.6285; binomial test, P=0.073, n=12) greater proportion of matching shells chosen (Fig. 2A). No difference was observed in the timing of initial shell choices (median time 278 s; Kruskal–Wallis, P=0.9565) or initial choices of matching shells (median time=469 s; Kruskal–Wallis, P=0.6594) (see Figs S1–4). In looming experiments, animals responded significantly more often (binomial test, P<1E−3, n=11) than to the negative control (Weber contrast=0.0) for all stimuli with contrasts stronger than −0.2, regardless of the background intensity (Fig. 2B). This indicates that although hermit crabs can perceive contrasts weaker than −0.45, they were not motivated to choose background-matching shells unless the visual contrast of the non-matching alternatives was relatively high.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Behavioural responses. (A) Fraction of choices for shells that matched the background. Open circles: first choices; filled circles: final choices. P-values are for a one-tailed binomial test against the chance level (0.5). (B) Proportion of responses in the startle experiments. Black: darker screen; grey: lighter screen. Filled symbols: significantly greater probability of response than with the control (Weber contrast=0) at the 5% level according to a binomial test; open symbols: non-significant. Weber contrast values are negative as all shells and stimuli were darker than the background. Error bars show 95% Wilson score intervals.

Previous studies have demonstrated the propensity of hermit crabs to choose shells or substrates so as to reduce their contrast. In the study by Partridge (1980), shells and backgrounds were painted black or white; no measurements of these shades were reported, but it can be safely assumed that these were of relatively high contrast to the hermit crabs' vision. In the work of Briffa et al. (2008), naturally coloured shells and sand substrates were photographed and Michelson contrast was calculated. Weber and Michelson contrasts are not easily compared, and uncalibrated camera images do not give good estimates of perceived contrast from the perspective of non-human visual systems (Stevens et al., 2007). However, the shells in that study were probably also of high contrast.

Here, our results corroborate the finding that given the option between highly contrasting or minimally contrasting shells, hermit crabs select shells that match the background. The results also allow us to quantitatively estimate the extent to which hermit crabs are motivated to perform this behaviour.

There are limitations in making comparisons between shell choice and startle experiments and differences in the contrast sensitivity of behavioural responses may be context dependent. For example, startle responses are driven by motion cues whereas shell choice is a slow response to a static stimulus (Tomsic, 2016).

In conclusion, hermit crabs prefer shells that match the background closely in reflectance, although they are not motivated to switch to a matching shell below a certain degree of contrast, even though they are seen to be sensitive to lower contrast values. This indicates that they make an assessment of the trade-off between the risks of moving between shells and the benefits of improved camouflage.

Hermit crabs present interesting study species in which to examine the concept of self-selection of camouflage, as they not only choose their substrate but also have both the opportunity and the capacity to maximise their crypsis behaviourally. The synthetic shell paradigm presented here controls for other aspects of shells that can influence choice, such as scent, size and weight, allowing more powerful assessment of the visually guided components of behavioural crypsis, and may be used in the future to test other aspects of self-selected camouflage including chromatic information and patterning.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Almut Kelber, Peter Olsson, Nick Roberts, Martin How and Innes Cuthill for useful discussions during the development of the project, and Sam Smithers for animal collection assistance and useful discussions. 3D-printed shells were inspired by TeamTeamUSA's Project Shellter. Part of this work was conducted as S.R.’s Masters research in the School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol.

FOOTNOTES

  • Competing interests

    The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

  • Author contributions

    Conceptualization: D.W.; Methodology: D.W., I.M.D.; Software: D.W., I.M.D.; Validation: D.W., J.J.F.; Formal analysis: D.W., J.J.F.; Investigation: D.W., S.R., A.B., M.W.; Data curation: D.W.; Writing - original draft: D.W., I.M.D., J.J.F.; Writing - review & editing: D.W., I.M.D., J.J.F.; Visualization: D.W.; Supervision: D.W.; Project administration: D.W.

  • Funding

    D.W. was funded by the Leverhulme Trust (RPG-2014-363 to Nicholas Roberts). J.J.F. is grateful for funding from Carl Tryggers Stiftelse för Vetenskaplig Forskning (15:108).

  • Data availability

    Data are available from the University of Bristol data repository, data.bris: https://doi.org/10.5523/bris.3uq1m3hcswj2f2h6yrd94to4p7.

  • Supplementary information

    Supplementary information available online at http://jeb.biologists.org/lookup/doi/10.1242/jeb.173831.supplemental

  • Received November 6, 2017.
  • Accepted May 15, 2018.
  • © 2018. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd
http://www.biologists.com/user-licence-1-1/

References

  1. ↵
    1. Ahnesjö, J. and
    2. Forsman, A.
    (2006). Differential habitat selection by pygmy grasshopper color morphs; interactive effects of temperature and predator avoidance. Evol. Ecol. 20, 235-257. doi:10.1007/s10682-006-6178-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    1. Briffa, M.,
    2. Haskell, P. and
    3. Wilding, C.
    (2008). Behavioural colour change in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus: reduced crypticity when the threat of predation is high. Behaviour 145, 915-929. doi:10.1163/156853908784089261
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Cortie, M. B.
    (1993). Digital seashells. Computer & Graphics 17, 79-84. doi:10.1016/0097-8493(93)90054-D
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Cronin, T. W. and
    2. Forward, R. B.
    (1988). The visual pigments of crabs. J. Comp. Physiol. A 162, 463-478. doi:10.1007/BF00612512
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    1. Gillis, J. E.
    (1982). Substrate colour-matching cues in the cryptic grasshopper Circotettix rabula rabula (Rehn & Hebard). Anim. Behav. 30, 113-116. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80244-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    1. Govardovskii, V. I.,
    2. Fyhrquist, N.,
    3. Reuter, T.,
    4. Kuzmin, D. G. and
    5. Donner, K.
    (2000). In search of the visual pigment template. Vis. Neurosci. 17, 509-528. doi:10.1017/S0952523800174036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    1. Gravel, B. E.,
    2. Wong, P. Y.,
    3. Starks, P. T. and
    4. Pechenik, J. A.
    (2004). The use of artificial shells for exploring shell preference in the marine hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus (Say). Ann. Zool. Fenn. 41, 477-485.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. Hanlon, R.
    (2007). Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Curr. Biol. 17, R400-R404. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. ↵
    1. Kelber, A.,
    2. Vorobyev, M. and
    3. Osorio, D.
    (2003). Animal colour vision – behavioural tests and physiological concepts. Biol. Rev. 78, 81-118. doi:10.1017/S1464793102005985
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Kettlewell, H. B. D. and
    2. Conn, D. L. T.
    (1977). Further background choice experiments on cryptic Lepidoptera. J. Zool. 181, 371-376. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1977.tb03250.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    1. Lancaster, I.
    (1988). Pagurus bernhardus (L.)–an introduction to the natural history of hermit crabs. Field Stud. 7, 189-238.
    OpenUrl
  12. ↵
    1. Marshall, K. L. A.,
    2. Philpot, K. E. and
    3. Stevens, M.
    (2016). Microhabitat choice in island lizards enhances camouflage against avian predators. Sci. Rep. 6, 19815. doi:10.1038/srep19815
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Merilaita, S. and
    2. Jormalainen, V.
    (1997). Evolution of sex differences in microhabitat choice and colour polymorphism in Idotea baltica. Anim. Behav. 54, 769-778. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0490
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  14. ↵
    1. Mesce, K. A.
    (1982). Calcium-bearing objects elicit shell selection behaviour in a hermit crab. Science 215, 993-995. doi:10.1126/science.215.4535.993
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Partridge, B. L.
    (1980). Background camouflage: an additional parameter in hermit crab shell selection and subsequent behaviour. Bull. Mar. Sci. 30, 914-916.
    OpenUrl
  16. R Core Team (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/.
  17. ↵
    1. Reese, E. S.
    (1963). The behavioral mechanisms underlying shell selection by hermit crabs. Behaviour 21, 78-126. doi:10.1163/156853963X00121
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. R Studio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, Inc. Boston, MA. URL http://www.rstudio.com/.
  19. ↵
    1. Shragai, T.,
    2. Ping, X.,
    3. Arakaki, C.,
    4. Garlick, D.,
    5. Blumstein, D. T. and
    6. Blaisdell, A. P.
    (2017). Hermit crab response to a visual threat is sensitive to looming cues. PeerJ 5, e4058. doi:10.7717/peerj.4058
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Smithers, S. P.,
    2. Wilson, A. and
    3. Stevens, M.
    (2017). Rock pool gobies change their body pattern in response to background features. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 121, 109-121. doi:10.1093/biolinnean/blw022
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    1. Stevens, M.,
    2. Párraga, C. A.,
    3. Cuthill, I. C.,
    4. Partridge, J. C. and
    5. Troscianko, T. S.
    (2007). Using digital photography to study animal coloration. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 90, 211-237. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00725.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  22. ↵
    1. Stieve, H.
    (1960). Die Spektrale Empfindichkeitskurve des Augesvon Eupagurus bernhardus. Zeit. Vergleichende Phys. 43, 518-525. doi:10.1007/BF00298074
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Stuart-Fox, D.,
    2. Whiting, M. J. and
    3. Moussalli, A.
    (2006). Camouflage and colour change: antipredator responses to bird and snake predators across multiple populations in a dwarf chameleon. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 88, 437-446. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00631.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    1. Tomsic, D.
    (2016). Visual motion processing subserving behavior in crabs. Curr. Op. Neurobiol. 41, 113-121. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2016.09.003
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Tyrie, E. K.,
    2. Hanlon, R. T.,
    3. Siemann, L. A. and
    4. Uyarra, M. C.
    (2014). Coral reef flounders, Bothus lunatus, choose substrates on which they can achieve camouflage with their limited body pattern repertoire. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 114, 629-638. doi:10.1111/bij.12442
    OpenUrlCrossRef
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • Colouration
  • Background matching
  • Crypsis
  • Vision

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) use visual contrast in self-assessment of camouflage
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) use visual contrast in self-assessment of camouflage
David Wilby, Samuel Riches, Ilse M. Daly, Andrew Bird, Matthew Wheelwright, James J. Foster
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb173831 doi: 10.1242/jeb.173831 Published 4 July 2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) use visual contrast in self-assessment of camouflage
David Wilby, Samuel Riches, Ilse M. Daly, Andrew Bird, Matthew Wheelwright, James J. Foster
Journal of Experimental Biology 2018 221: jeb173831 doi: 10.1242/jeb.173831 Published 4 July 2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgements
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Strain-specific differences in muscle Ca2+ transport and mitochondrial electron transport chain proteins between FVB/N and C57BL/6J mice
  • Latencies of mechanically-stimulated escape responses in the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi
  • Rapid toxin sequestration modifies poison frog physiology
Show more SHORT COMMUNICATION

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


Critical temperature window sends migratory black-headed buntings on their travels

The spring rise in temperature at black-headed bunting overwintering sites is essential for triggering the physical changes that they undergo before embarking on their spring migration – read more.


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992