Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Research Article
Individual variation in metabolic reaction norms over ambient temperature causes low correlation between basal and standard metabolic rate
Michael Briga, Simon Verhulst
Journal of Experimental Biology 2017 220: 3280-3289; doi: 10.1242/jeb.160069
Michael Briga
Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Nijenborgh 7, 9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Michael Briga
  • For correspondence: michbriga@gmail.com
Simon Verhulst
Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Nijenborgh 7, 9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Basal metabolic rate (BMR) is often assumed to be indicative of the energy turnover at ambient temperatures (Ta) below the thermoneutral zone (SMR), but this assumption has remained largely untested. Using a new statistical approach, we quantified the consistency in nocturnal metabolic rate across a temperature range in zebra finches (N=3213 measurements on 407 individuals) living permanently in eight outdoor aviaries. Foraging conditions were either benign or harsh, and body mass and mass-adjusted BMR (BMRm) and SMR (SMRm) were lower in individuals living in a harsh foraging environment. The correlation between SMRm at different Ta was high (r=0.91), independent of foraging environment, showing that individuals are consistently ranked according to their SMRm. However, the correlations between BMRm and SMRm were always lower (average: r=0.29; range: 0<r<0.50), in particular in the benign foraging environment. Variation in metabolic response to lower Ta at least in part reflected differential body temperature (Tb) regulation: early morning Tb was lower at low Ta, and more so in individuals with a weaker metabolic response to lower Ta. Our findings have implications for the use of BMR in the estimation of time–energy budgets and comparative analyses: we suggest that the use of metabolic rates at ecologically relevant Ta, such as the easily tractable SMR, will be more informative than the use of BMR as a proxy for energy turnover.

INTRODUCTION

Energy is an essential resource for reproduction and survival (Boutin, 1990; Martin, 1987; Prevedello et al., 2013; Ruffino et al., 2014). The energy an individual spends daily on all activities is called the daily energy expenditure (DEE) and includes all processes such as self-maintenance, thermoregulation and behaviour. Measuring DEE is highly relevant but often practically demanding. A component of energy turnover that is more tractable and often quantified is basal metabolic rate (BMR), i.e. the minimum energy expenditure of a post-absorptive adult animal measured during the rest phase at thermoneutral temperature (IUPS Thermal Commission, 2001; McNab, 1997). Thermoneutral temperature is defined as the ambient temperature (Ta) at which body temperature (Tb) regulation is achieved without regulatory changes in metabolic heat production or evaporative water loss (IUPS Thermal Commission, 2001). BMR has been studied in association with many traits such as growth, reproduction, personality, oxidative stress, senescence and survival (reviewed in Biro and Stamps, 2010; Burton et al., 2011; Glazier, 2015). It is often implicitly assumed that individual variation in BMR is representative of individual variation in DEE. However, the correlation between DEE and BMR is generally weak in birds and mammals (0<R2<0.23; Careau et al., 2012; Fyhn et al., 2001; Meerlo et al., 1997; Speakman et al., 2003; Tieleman et al., 2008; Wiersma and Tinbergen, 2003). Thus, the assumption that individual variation in BMR reflects variation in DEE, and hence can be interpreted as an index of total energy turnover, is not well supported.

Multiple hypotheses can be formulated to explain why BMR and DEE are only weakly correlated. The hypothesis we investigated here is that the low correlation between BMR and DEE is at least in part due to the fact that BMR is measured at thermoneutrality, while DEE is measured at Ta as experienced in natural environments, which are often below thermoneutrality. As BMR represents a considerable proportion of an individual's energy expenditure (∼30%; e.g. Careau et al., 2012; Daan et al., 1990), we would expect a positive association between BMR and standard metabolic rate (SMR), i.e. metabolic rate below thermoneutrality but otherwise in identical conditions. However, SMR is more influenced than BMR by Tb and insulation. These could associate positively with BMR, for example because individuals with poor insulation need an enhanced thermoregulatory machinery to maintain Tb, causing an indirect positive association between BMR and SMR. Conversely, Tb and insulation might differ between individuals to such an extent that BMR and SMR will correlate poorly. To the best of our knowledge, the association between BMR and SMR remains unknown. To investigate individual variation in this relationship, we repeatedly measured BMR and SMR in a small passerine, the zebra finch, in the same individuals and under the same conditions except that Ta was below the thermoneutral zone during SMR measurements. If individual differences in thermoregulatory response to a lower Ta are small relative to the average response, individual variation in BMR will be strongly correlated with SMR. In this case, BMR and SMR can be considered different expressions of the ‘same’ trait. Alternatively, individuals may differ in their thermoregulatory response to the extent that the correlation between BMR and SMR is weak or absent. In this case, BMR and SMR are uncoupled and this would at least partly explain the low correlations observed between BMR and DEE.

When multiple traits of an individual are measured multiple times, phenotypic correlations between traits can arise via two ways (Fig. 1). Firstly, individual mean values of trait A may correlate with individual mean values of trait B, which we here define as between-individual correlations. Secondly, the change over time in trait A may correlate with the change in trait B in that same individual, which we here define as within-individual correlations (following Dingemanse and Dochtermann, 2013). Because correlations at the two levels reflect biologically distinct processes, we decomposed the phenotypic correlations between BMR and SMR into between- versus within-individual correlations. Because we were mostly interested in differences between individuals, we discuss here mainly between-individual correlations.

List of abbreviations
BMR
basal metabolic rate
BMRm
mass-adjusted basal metabolic rate
DEE
daily energy expenditure
MMR
maximum metabolic rate
SMR
standard metabolic rate
SMRm
mass-adjusted standard metabolic rate
Ta
ambient temperature
Tb
body temperature

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Graphic representation of between- and within-individual correlations for two traits A and B. Each arrow represents an individual with a first and second measurement (open and filled circles, respectively). A high between-individual correlation means that individual mean values of trait A correlate with individual mean values of trait B (bottom two illustrations). A high within-individual correlation means that for a particular individual the change in value of trait A correlates with the change in value of trait B (i.e. arrows are in similar directions for all individuals, illustrations on the right).

Potential correlations between variables are affected by the variables' repeatability. For example, when repeatability of a trait is zero, between-individual correlations with that trait will also be zero. Furthermore, repeatability is relevant in evolutionary terms because consistent differences between individuals are a minimum requirement for natural selection to act upon (Falconer and Mackay, 1996). Hence, here we first quantified the repeatability of body mass, BMR and SMR of zebra finches at Ta ranging from 5 to 39°C (Fig. 2). We then investigated the within- and between-individual correlations between metabolic rates at different Ta. When exposed to a lower Ta, homoeothermic organisms balance three interrelated physiological factors: metabolic rate, insulation and Tb (Geiser, 2004; McNab, 1980). For example, large differences between Ta and Tb increase heat loss, and one way to minimize this loss is by down-regulating Tb (Angilletta et al., 2010; Geiser, 2004; Körtner et al., 2000). Thus, Tb adjustments can be an important determinant of metabolic responses to lower Ta. To investigate the role of temperature regulation, we measured Tb of a subset of individuals at multiple Ta and correlated individual metabolic reaction norms over Ta with changes in Tb.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Metabolic rate declines with increasing ambient temperature (Ta­) until the thermoneutral zone between 32 and 39°C. Data are consistent with the classic results of Scholander et al. (1950) and Calder (1964). The grey line shows the fit. N=3213 measurements from 407 individuals.

Repeatability and trait correlations are inherently specific to a population and its environment. The birds used in this study lived in captivity, which may alter metabolism and possibly its repeatability relative to that of free-living animals (e.g. Auer et al., 2016). One essential difference between captive and free-living populations is that food can usually be accessed at negligible costs in captivity, which is not usually true for free-living animals (Beaulieu, 2016; Briga and Verhulst, 2015a), and this can have physiological and demographic consequences (e.g. Briga et al., 2017; Robb et al., 2008). To broaden the range of environments and increase the ecological relevance of our study, we housed the birds in outdoor aviaries, and permanently exposed half of our population to high foraging costs through a manipulation of flight cost per food reward (Koetsier and Verhulst, 2011). Increased foraging costs generally result in lower BMR (reviewed in Wiersma and Verhulst, 2005), and in zebra finches this effect was stronger at lower temperatures, i.e. on SMR (Wiersma and Verhulst, 2005). However, whether foraging costs affect the association between BMR and SMR is unknown. Thus, we experimentally manipulated foraging costs and compared BMR and SMR repeatability and correlations in a ‘benign’ environment versus a ‘harsh’ semi-natural environment.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Birds and housing

The birds we studied are part of a long-term experiment investigating the relationships between foraging costs and survival (Briga and Verhulst, 2015b; Briga et al., 2017). Birds were housed in eight unisex outdoor aviaries (L×W×H: 320×150×225 cm) located in Groningen, The Netherlands (53°13′0″N, 6°33′0″E). Foraging costs were manipulated as described by Koetsier and Verhulst (2011). In brief, in each aviary a food box was attached to the ceiling, with holes in the sides from which food (tropical seed mixture) could be obtained. In the benign foraging environment (four aviaries) the food box had perches beneath the holes, while in the harsh foraging environment (also four aviaries) these were removed, forcing birds to fly and hover for seeds. Water, grit and cuttlebone were provided ad libitum and birds received 1.25 g of fortified canary food (‘eggfood’, Bogena, Hedel, The Netherlands) per individual per week given in three portions. Each aviary contained an approximately equal number of birds (15–25) and we kept bird density within a limited range by regularly adding birds to replace those that died. All birds had been reared in either small or large broods with in most cases two or six young. The manipulated brood sizes were within the natural range for zebra finches in the wild (Zann, 1996) and in captivity (Griffith et al., 2017). The brood size manipulation did not affect mass-adjusted BMR/SMR (Briga, 2016) and will therefore not be further considered here. All methods and experimental protocols were carried out under the approval of the Animal Experimentation Ethical Committee of the University of Groningen, licence 5150A. All methods were carried out in accordance with these approved guidelines.

Body mass and body size

Between December 2007, when the experiment started, and December 2015, we collected 15,443 mass measurements on 597 individuals. Birds were weighed almost monthly, up to 95 times (Fig. S1A) between the ages of 0.4 and 9.4 years. We used this whole dataset to estimate treatment effects. However, to consistently estimate repeatability, within- and between-individual variances (see below) based on the same individuals, we selected those individuals with at least two measurements (N=15,418 measurements on 572 individuals). Size measurements, tarsus and head+bill, were taken after reaching maturity, on average at age 133±33 days (mean±s.d.), and averaged after transformation to a standard normal distribution to obtain one estimate of structural body size.

Metabolic rate

Overnight energy expenditure was measured using an open-flow respirometer situated in a dark climate-controlled room kept at the desired Ta. Up to 16 individuals per night were taken from the aviaries on average at 18:10 h (±1:17 h s.d.), weighed (±0.1 g) and randomly assigned to one of 16, 1.5 l metabolic chambers in a dark climate room. Measurements lasted until the morning, such that birds were in a post-absorptive state, thus meeting the requirements for BMR (IUPS Thermal Commission, 2001; McNab, 1997) and this was consistent for SMR as well. Rooms were kept and continuously monitored at the above-mentioned temperatures with multiple PT100 temperature sensors, one located in the room recording continuously and one in each metabolic chamber recording at each metabolic rate measurement. Technical specification of the equipment can be found in Bouwhuis et al. (2011). In brief, the air flow through the metabolic chambers was controlled at 25 l h−1 by mass-flow controllers (5850S, Brooks, Rijswijk, The Netherlands) calibrated with a bubble flow meter. Air was dried using a molecular sieve (3 Å; Merck, Darmstadt, Germany) and analysed by a paramagnetic oxygen analyser (Servomex Xentra 4100, Crowborough, UK). During measurements, each metabolic chamber or reference outdoor air was sampled every 8 min for 60 s to stabilize measurement levels. In each sampling, we measured O2 concentration and oxygen consumption was calculated using equation 6 of Hill (1972). An energy equivalent of 19.7 kJ l−1 oxygen consumed was used to calculate energy expenditure in watts. Metabolic rate was taken to be the minimum value of a 30 min running average, which included 3–6 measurements per individual. The first measurement hour was excluded to minimize potential effects of handling stress and the incomplete mixing of air in the metabolic chamber. Birds were weighed before and after the respirometry measurement and body mass for the metabolic rate analyses was taken to be the average of the two values.

Between December 2007 and April 2013, we collected 3213 metabolic rate measurements from 407 birds. Metabolic rate measurements were obtained at Ta ranging from 5 to 39°C (Fig. 2), but most measurements were centred on three Ta of 36°C (range 32–39°C, Fig. S2) for BMR and 26°C (±3°C) and 12°C (±3°C) for SMR (Table 1). We refer to the metabolic rates at these three Ta categories as BMR, SMR26 and SMR12, respectively. Measurements were concentrated in spring and autumn. Birds were measured up to 25 times (Fig. S1B) between the ages of 0.4 and 7.2 years. To estimate treatment and seasonal effects, we used this whole dataset, avoiding any possible bias by selecting data subsets. For the repeatability analyses, we used data subsets as described in Table 1. In brief, to estimate repeatability, within- and between-individual variances based on the same individuals and correlations, we selected those individuals with at least two measurements (Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Description of the metabolic rate dataset at ambient temperature (Ta) ranges for which most data were collected

Tb

During autumn–winter 2011, we measured Tb at the end of respirometry measurements (N=550; mean time 09:46 h, ±0:38 h s.d.) of 189 individuals using an Omega® Thermocouple Thermometer Type T smoothed with Johnson & Johnson® lubrication gel. Handling time during measurements was ≤30 s and the temperature reading was obtained within 5 s of the probe entering the cloaca, at which time Tb was stable. Tb increased as birds were subsequently measured in the climate-controlled room and hence we included measurement order as a covariate in all analyses. In analyses with Tb as predictor, we used order-adjusted values.

Statistical analyses

The repeatability is the proportion of phenotypic variance attributed to between-individual variance (Nakagawa and Schielzeth, 2010). These variance components can be estimated using a linear mixed model in which the between-individual variance is captured by including individual identity as a random effect and in which the phenotypic variance is the sum of the between-individual variance and the residual variance. In such models, residual variance decreases by including fixed effects, thereby increasing repeatability. Body mass repeatability estimates did not include fixed effects except when adjusting for size. Metabolic rate repeatability estimates included as linear fixed effects Ta and, for mass-adjusted estimates, mass. Variance components were estimated using a Bayesian approach (Dingemanse and Dochtermann, 2013) with the R package MCMCglmm (Hadfield, 2010) in R (www.R-project.org) with flat improper priors with 1.5×105 iterations, 10,000 burn-in and a thinning interval of 100. This yielded Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sample sizes of at least 1000 with low levels of autocorrelation (mean r=−0.002 with all r<0.1). Bayesian results were consistent with those of the frequentist approach (with restricted maximum likelihood and maximum likelihood; results not shown), using the functions (i) lmer of the package lme4 (Bates et al., 2015), (ii) lme of the package nlme (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme) and (iii) rpt of the package rptR (Nakagawa and Schielzeth, 2010). We here report Bayesian estimates with 95% credible intervals (CI). To test for significance of repeatability, we used likelihood ratio tests with the function exactLRT in the package RLRsim (Scheipl et al., 2008). To test for differences between repeatabilities, we used t-tests with the number of individual identities as (conservative) sample size. Covariation between traits was analysed using a Bayesian approach with a trivariate analysis (SMR12, SMR26 and BMR) with the function MCMCglmm (Hadfield, 2010) using uninformative inverse Wishart priors. In these models, metabolic values were residuals of a linear model with mass and Ta as fixed effects and with individual identity as a random effect. Repeatability and trait correlations for different foraging treatments were estimated separately by selecting data subsets. The effects of the season and foraging cost manipulation were analysed using general linear mixed models, lmer of the package lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) including individual as random effect. Residuals of all models were checked with function resid and all had a normal distribution without outliers. Effect sizes are reported as Cohen's d (Cohen, 1988), which was estimated as the ratio of the coefficient over the trait's standard deviation (equations 1 and 2 in Nakagawa and Cuthill, 2007). As a rule of thumb, effect sizes of 0.5 are considered as moderate (Cohen, 1988) and this was also the median effect size estimated from 43 ecological or evolutionary studies (Moller and Jennions, 2002).

RESULTS

Body mass repeatability

Repeatability of body mass was high at 0.72 (0.69<95% CI<0.74; Fig. 3B; N=15,418 measurements on 572 individuals). Body mass increased with size (r=0.56), and a modest part of the between-individual variation was due to variation in body size: body mass repeatability adjusted for size was 0.12 lower at 0.60 (0.57<95% CI<0.63; Fig. 3B). All estimates were significantly larger than zero (LR>11,324; P<10−15). Hence, the zebra finches in our population can be characterized by their body mass and size-adjusted body mass.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Environmental quality affects body mass but not its repeatability. (A) Body mass. (B) Repeatability of body mass, presented ±95% credible interval (CI). ‘Population’ refers to all the birds pooled; ‘Benign’ and ‘Harsh’ refer to the subset of the population that lived in either the benign or harsh environment (low and high foraging cost, respectively). Boxplots show median, 25–75 and 5–95 percentiles (black vertical lines) and points outside this range. Repeatability estimates are based on the between- and within-individual variances in Table S1. Note, the smaller body mass range in the harsh environment in A, which was due to smaller within- and between-individual variance (Table S1).

Birds exposed to high foraging costs weighed on average 15.0 g, which was 4% (0.64 g, effect size d=0.41) lighter than birds with low foraging costs (Fig. 3A; F1,580=43, P<10−8), and this difference persisted when controlling for size (F1,471=44, P<10−10). Thus, high foraging costs negatively affected body mass.

Environmental conditions can affect between- and within-individual variance of traits, potentially making repeatability values conditional on the environment. The mass of birds living with high foraging costs was characterized by smaller between- and within-individual variance relative to birds living with low foraging costs (Table S1; t469>2.56, P<0.011). However, between- and within-individual variance components changed to the same extent, and hence repeatability estimates of body mass and size-adjusted body mass were similar in the two environments (∼0.70 and 0.60, respectively; Fig. 3B). Thus, environmental quality did not affect the repeatability of body mass, but individuals in a harsh environment experienced smaller body mass variation between and within individuals.

Metabolic rate repeatability

Metabolic rate decreased from 5 to 32°C (Fig. 2), consistent with the classic literature (Scholander et al., 1950). Note that with decreasing metabolic rate, s.d. also decreased (Table 1). This decrease in s.d. was proportional to the decrease in mean value as the coefficients of variation remained similar across all Ta (Table 1). Between 32 and 39°C, metabolic rate was steady (Fig. 2). We identified this Ta range as the zebra finches’ thermoneutral zone (Fig. S2), confirming earlier results (Calder, 1964) with a 10-fold larger dataset. Thus, the associations between metabolic rate and Ta were consistent with those described in the classic literature.

Repeatability of whole-organism BMR was 0.54 (0.47<95% CI<0.58; LR=379, P<10−15; Fig. 4B; Table S2), within the range of previously published results (Nespolo and Franco, 2007; Versteegh et al., 2008; White et al., 2013). Whole-organism metabolic rate is to a large extent determined by body mass (r=0.61). When body mass was added to the statistical model, repeatability of mass-adjusted BMR (BMRm) was halved to 0.27 (0.22<95% CI<0.35; LR=94, P<10−15; Fig. 4B; Table S2). Similarly, for SMR, the repeatability of whole-organism values was 0.39 (0.33<95% CI<0.43; LR=451, P<10−15), which was larger than that for mass-adjusted values (SMRm), which was 0.28 (0.24<95% CI<0.35; LR=248, P<10−15; Fig. 4B; Table S2). The repeatability of BMRm and SMRm was very similar at 0.27 and 0.28, respectively (Fig. 4B; Table S2). For SMRm within narrower Ta ranges (SMRm12 and SMRm26), the repeatability was slightly higher at 0.30 and 0.40, respectively (Fig. 4B; both LR>102, P<10−15). Thus, the zebra finches in our population can be characterized by their BMRm as well as by their SMRm. Because the high repeatability of body mass often inflates metabolic rate repeatability (Fig. 4B,D,E), we further discuss results for mass-adjusted values only. Whole-organism results were mostly identical (results not shown).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Mass-adjusted metabolic rate (MRm) and its repeatability for different subsets of the data. MRm increased at lower Ta (SMR at 12 versus 26°C; A), but individuals can be equally well characterized by their mass-adjusted basal metabolic rate (BMRm) as by their mass-adjusted standard metabolic rate (SMRm; B). MRm and its repeatability were higher in spring than in autumn (C,D) and in benign than in harsh environments (E,F). Mass-adjusted values were estimated for the population mean body mass of 15.13 g. Boxplots show median, and 25–75 and 5–95 (black vertical lines) percentiles. Differences in repeatability estimates can arise as a result of changes in either between- or within-individual variance; these are summarized in Table S2.

In accordance with earlier studies in non-migratory birds (McKechnie, 2008), we found BMRm and SMRm to vary seasonally. BMRm and SMRm were, respectively, 0.011 and 0.025 W g−1 higher in spring than in autumn (Fig. 4C; BMRm: F1,1001=108, P<10−15; SMRm: F1,1880=153, P<10−15), or an effect size d=0.5 for both traits. Seasonal mass-adjusted metabolic rate change within individuals may exceed that between individuals (e.g. Bouwhuis et al., 2011). This would result in higher metabolic rate repeatability within season (in different years) compared with the mass-adjusted metabolic rate repeatability estimated with seasons pooled. For BMRm, spring and autumn repeatability were within the same range as year-round repeatability (0.26 and 0.18 versus 0.27, respectively; Fig. 4D; t443<1.46, P>0.14). For SMRm, autumn repeatability was within the same range as year-round repeatability (0.24 versus 0.28; Fig. 4D), but spring repeatability was higher (0.40; Fig. 4D; t539=2.6, P<0.0095). Thus, birds showed seasonal adjustments in metabolic rate and these adjustments were consistent between individuals except for SMRm in spring.

Increases in foraging effort often lead to energy-saving decreases in BMRm (reviewed in Wiersma and Verhulst, 2005). Indeed, BMRm and SMRm were lower in birds experiencing higher foraging costs (Fig. 4E; BMRm: F1,338=28, P<10−6; SMRm: F1,271=82, P<10−15), with the effect being more pronounced on SMRm (0.030 W g−1 or an effect size d=0.60) than on BMRm (0.009 W g−1 or an effect size d=0.42; Fig. 4E; F1,3002=112, P<10−15). The negative effect of foraging costs on SMRm also became more pronounced at lower Ta (F1,1692=4.7, P=0.03). Thus, birds from harsh environments lowered their minimal energy expenditure, and this became more pronounced with colder Ta.

Repeatability of BMRm and SMRm was, respectively, 23% and 48% higher in the low foraging cost environment than in the high foraging cost environment (BMRm: 0.26 versus 0.20; SMRm: 0.31 versus 0.16; Fig. 4F; Table S2), but these differences were at best marginally significant (t309<1.83, P>0.07). For BMRm, the lower repeatability arose as a result of lower between-individual variance in the high foraging cost environment, while the within-individual variance was environment independent (Table S2). For SMRm, the high foraging cost birds were characterized by lower between- and within-individual variance, but none of these patterns were significant (Table S2; t309<1.60, P>0.11). Thus, birds in a benign environment had a higher repeatability of metabolic rate, but the effect of environment was not significant.

Metabolic rate correlations and reaction norms at multiple Ta

The phenotypic correlation between SMRm12 and SMRm26 was moderate at 0.43 (Table 2; Fig. S3C; t577>11.45, P<10−15). Surprisingly, the phenotypic correlations between BMRm and either SMRm12 or SMRm26 were substantially lower (0.14<r<0.22; Table 2; Fig. S3C; t577>3.46, P<0.0006). These patterns were consistent in both foraging environments (Table 2). Thus, SMRm values at different Ta correlate better with each other than with BMRm.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Correlations (±95% credible interval) between mass-adjusted metabolic rates at multiple levels

Phenotypic correlations in datasets with multiple observations per individual are the combined result of between- and within-individual correlations (Fig. 1) and here we tease apart these components. The between-individual correlation between SMRm12 and SMRm26 was higher than the phenotypic correlation and remained close to 1 in both foraging cost groups (0.80<r<0.91; Table 2). In contrast, the within-individual correlation between SMRm12 and SMRm26 was low (0.04<r<0.25; Table 2) and significantly positive only in the low foraging cost group (Table 2). Hence, the phenotypic correlation between SMRm12 and SMRm26 arose as a result of high between-individual correlation. Thus, individuals can be ranked consistently by their mean SMRm over the whole sub-thermoneutral Ta range (Fig. 5A).

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

SMRm12 and SMRm26 associate better with each another than with BMRm. (A) Mass-adjusted metabolic rates of birds measured at three Ta (12, 26 and 36°C) and ordered along the x-axis according to increasing mass-adjusted metabolic rate at 12°C (SMRm12). The consistent increase in metabolic rate to the right of the x-axis for SMRm12 and for SMRm26 illustrates their repeatability and between-individual correlation. This increase to the right is not present for BMRm, showing its weak correlation with any of the SMRm values. (B) Illustration of (mean) individual reaction norms for 30 randomly selected birds. Note that reaction norms appear more parallel below than above 32°C.

In contrast with the findings for SMRm12 and SMRm26, correlations between BMRm and any of the SMRm values were weak and their 95% CI often overlapped with zero between and within individuals and in both foraging cost groups (Table 2). Hence, the weak phenotypic correlations between BMRm and any of SMRm values arose as a result of weak between- and weak within-individual correlations. Thus, the ranking of individuals according to their mean BMRm differs from the ranking according to their mean SMRm (Fig. 5A).

The high correlation between SMRm12 and SMRm26 indicates that individuals can be characterized by their metabolic rate reaction norms over sub-thermoneutral Ta. Statistically, an individual reaction norm can be quantified by a random slope. We hence ran a random slope model with SMR data, including Ta and mass as fixed effects and individual identity as a random effect and we quantified the random slope by nesting Ta in individual identity. Indeed, a random slope model fitted the data better than a random intercept model for both SMR (ΔAICc=−41) and SMRm (ΔAICc=−15; Fig. 5B). Thus, metabolic reaction norms over sub-thermoneutral Ta varied significantly between individuals. The improvement in model fit by inclusion of random slopes was larger in the low foraging cost environment than in the high foraging cost environment (SMR: ΔAICc=−6.8; SMRm: ΔAICc=−3.3). Thus, individuals can be characterized by their metabolic reaction norms over sub-thermoneutral Ta (Fig. 5B) and this is most pronounced in a benign environment.

Tb

Birds decreased their Tb in response to lower Ta (Fig. 6; F2,314=313, P<10−15). Previous studies have shown that Tb is lower in individuals from harsh foraging environments (reviewed in Geiser, 2004; Vuarin and Henry, 2014). We confirmed this pattern at Ta=12°C (Fig. 6; F1,133=9.24, P=0.0029) and at thermoneutral Ta (Ta=36°C; Fig. 6; F1,147=9.22, P=0.0028), but not at Ta=26°C (Fig. 6; F1,124=0.36, P=0.55). The environment×Ta interaction was significant (F2,350=3.36, P=0.036). Thus, individuals in harsh environments maintained either the same or lower night-time Tb depending on Ta.

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Tb is lower at lower Ta and this is more pronounced for birds in the harsh foraging environment. Boxplots show median, and 25–75 and 5–95 (black vertical lines) percentiles, N=550 measurements on 189 individuals.

There are multiple solutions to balancing metabolic rate, insulation and Tb (McNab, 1980). Thermal physics predicts that, everything else remaining equal, individuals with low SMRm also have a lower Tb (McNab, 1980). Indeed, this was the case at Ta=12°C (Fig. S4A; r=0.22; F1,188=9.0, P=0.003). However, this association was absent at Ta=26°C (Fig. S4A; F1,124=0.09, P=0.77) and for BMR (Fig. S4A; r=0.05; F1,172=0.11, P=0.74). These results were consistent for both foraging cost groups (F1,122<2.63, P>0.11). The difference in the association between Tb and mass-adjusted metabolic rate at different Ta values was significant (F2,481=3.04, P=0.05), with the association becoming stronger with decreasing Ta.

We next investigated whether the weak between-individual correlation between BMRm and SMRm was associated with individual differences in Tb response to sub-thermoneutral Ta. We tested whether individual responses of mass-adjusted metabolic rate and Tb to colder Ta were correlated. Indeed, when comparing mass-adjusted metabolic rate and Tb between Ta of 36 and 12°C, individuals with the largest increases in mass-adjusted metabolic rate maintained the highest Tb (Fig. 7; r=0.21; F1,121=5.46, P=0.02) for both foraging cost groups (Fig. 7; F1,119=0.29, P=0.59). Between 26 and 12°C, we found a significant association for birds in the low foraging cost environment (Fig. S4B; F1,59=7.45, P=0.008), but not for birds in the high foraging cost environment (Fig. S4B; F1,41=0.16, P=0.69) and this difference was significant (F1,101=4.34, P=0.04). Between 36 and 26°C, we found little evidence for positive association between mass-adjusted metabolic rate and Tb responses (Fig. S4C; r=−0.24; F1,84=2.56, P=0.11). Hence, we found some evidence that when facing sub-thermoneutral Ta, some individuals maintain a higher mass-adjusted metabolic rate and Tb than others. Thus, the weak between-individual correlations between BMRm and SMRm (Table 2) are in part due to individual differences in Tb regulation in response to sub-thermoneutral Ta.

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

Individuals with a smaller difference in metabolic rate between 12 and 36°C show a larger difference in Tb. Grey circles and dashed line show data and model fit for the benign environment; black triangles and continuous line show data and model fit for the harsh environment.

DISCUSSION

Body mass, BMRm and SMRm were repeatable traits in our study population (Fig. 3B, Fig. 4B). SMRm at different Ta correlated almost perfectly between individuals (Table 2, Fig. 5A,B). In contrast, correlations between BMRm and SMRm were always weaker (Table 2, Fig. 5A,B). Thus, individuals with high BMRm do not necessarily have high SMRm and our results better fit the scenario in Fig. S3B over that in Fig. S3A. Individual variation in metabolic reaction norms can have various causes (McNab, 1980) and here we showed a role for differential Tb regulation: individuals with the steeper metabolic reaction norms maintained a higher Tb at low ambient temperatures (Fig. 7). Thus, BMRm was a weak indicator of the energy turnover at sub-thermoneutral Ta and this was in part due to individual variation in Tb regulation.

Repeatability is likely to be environment specific, and thus the fact that our birds lived in captivity may have affected our findings. A major difference between captive and free-living populations is that food can often be accessed at negligible costs in captivity, which is not usually the case for free-living animals (e.g. Beaulieu, 2016; Briga and Verhulst, 2015a). To better mimic natural conditions, we therefore experimentally increased foraging costs, which decreased life expectancy up to 15% (Briga et al., 2017). A unique aspect of our study is that we carried out this manipulation for life, and hence our findings reflect long-term adjustments to foraging conditions. Individuals living with high foraging costs had lower body mass, BMRm and SMRm (Fig. 3A, Fig. 4A), consistent with shorter-term studies of birds (Bautista et al., 1998; Deerenberg et al., 1998; Koetsier and Verhulst, 2011; Wiersma and Verhulst, 2005) and mammals (Day and Bartness, 2001; Perrigo, 1987; Schubert et al., 2009; Vaanholt et al., 2007). The standardized effect size d of foraging costs on mass-adjusted metabolic rate was approximately 0.5, which appears a reasonably large effect (Moller and Jennions, 2002). However, to establish the biological significance of this finding, we need to know how the observed effect sizes translate into differences in lifespan and/or reproductive success.

The effect of increased foraging costs was more pronounced on SMRm than on BMRm, in agreement with the findings of Wiersma and Verhulst (2005). The increasing effect of foraging costs with lower temperature is probably due to increased use of energy-saving mechanisms at lower Ta, such as night-time hypothermia (Fig. 6), which appears to be a general response to increased foraging costs or food shortage (Angilletta et al., 2010; Daan et al., 1989; Geiser, 2004; McKechnie and Lovegrove, 2002; Vuarin and Henry, 2014). That more energy is saved when foraging costs are increased is not surprising, because an increase in thermoregulatory requirements leads to a knock-on increase in energy expenditure in hard foraging conditions, because more energy needs to be expended to gather the extra energy for thermoregulation. Thus, we predict that diurnal energy expenditure will increase faster in response to decreasing ambient temperature when foraging costs are high, but this remains to be tested.

Repeatability

The repeatability of whole-organism metabolic rate in the range 0.4–0.5 found here are consistent with the existing literature, with most repeatabilities ranging between 0.3 and 0.8 (Auer et al., 2016; Nespolo and Franco, 2007; Versteegh et al., 2008; White et al., 2013). Our repeatability of mass-adjusted metabolic rate, ranging between 0.3 and 0.4, is also consistent with earlier studies in birds, although perhaps on the lower range relative to that found in earlier zebra finch studies, which was between 0.3 and 0.6 (Careau et al., 2014; Rønning et al., 2005; Verhulst et al., 2006; Vézina and Williams, 2005). Two aspects of our study are likely to have contributed to this difference. Firstly, our dataset covers a larger time range (up to 5.5 years), which deflates trait repeatability (Auer et al., 2016; White et al., 2013). Secondly, our birds were housed outdoors and thus exposed to a wider range of environmental variation than birds housed indoors. Thus, the repeatability of our metabolic rate measurements is within the range one would expect based on earlier studies.

How environmental quality affects trait repeatability is not well known. Heritability, i.e. the proportion of phenotypic variance due to additive genetic effects, was shown to increase weakly but significantly with environmental quality (Charmantier and Garant, 2005; Visscher et al., 2008; but see Rowinski and Rogell, 2017). Hence, a positive association is sometimes expected between repeatability and environmental quality. We did in fact find the expected difference for BMRm and SMRm, but the error around our estimates was such that this difference was only marginally significant despite a considerable sample size. However, for body mass and SMRm there was also an environmental effect on within-individual variance, being higher in the benign environment (significant for body mass, the trait with the largest sample size). This contrasts with the expectation for repeatability because, everything else remaining equal, large within-individual variance decreases repeatability. Indeed, the repeatability of body mass was independent of environmental quality because within- and between-individual variance both changed significantly and to the same extent. Our findings imply that there is no general prediction regarding the effect of environmental quality on trait repeatability.

Weak metabolic correlations: causes and implications

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to quantify the correlation between BMRm and SMRm. Intraspecific correlations between BMR (or BMRm) and other measures of energy expenditure, such as DEE and maximum metabolic rate (MMR; which is a special case of SMR when measured at very low Ta because it is not sustainable) were often found to be weak in endothermic species (DEE: Careau et al., 2012; Fyhn et al., 2001; Meerlo et al., 1997; Speakman et al., 2003; Tieleman et al., 2008; Wiersma and Tinbergen, 2003; cold-induced MMR: Chappell and Bachman, 1995; Petit et al., 2013; Swanson et al., 2012; Vézina et al., 2006; Wiersma et al., 2007a; Zhang et al., 2015). Why these correlations are weak is not well known. BMRm is largely determined by central organs, while insulation and Tb will in addition be important for energetic expenditure at sub-thermoneutral Ta (such as SMRm, DEE and MMR; Daan et al., 1989; Daan et al., 1990; Suarez and Darveau, 2005; Vézina et al., 2006; Wiersma et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015). That there are different drivers of variation in BMRm (central organ mass and cellular activity) and SMRm (insulation and Tb) is likely to contribute to weakening the correlation between BMRm and SMRm. Here, we showed that individual differences in thermoregulation (Tb) in response to low temperature caused a low correlation between BMRm and SMRm and the same effect is likely to weaken correlations with DEE and MMR. There are, however, more drivers of variation for the weak correlation between BMR and DEE or MMR; in particular, additional variance due to variation in activity level is likely to be important. The extent to which thermoregulation explains the weak correlation between BMR and DEE can be verified by testing how much better SMR at ecologically relevant temperatures correlates with DEE or MMR than BMR.

Between-individual correlations between SMRm12 and SMRm26 were high, and this observation was confirmed by the random slope analyses. In contrast, between-individual correlations between BMRm and SMRm were low. This finding may be due to individual differences in conductance (heat loss over the Tb–Ta gradient; McNab, 1980), with better insulated individuals showing a weaker metabolic rate response to lower Ta. Alternatively, the heterogeneity in metabolic rate response may be due to variation in Tb response to lower Ta. Our data (Figs 6 and 7) indicate that at least part of the heterogeneity in metabolic rate response can be attributed to the latter effect. Unfortunately, our data prevent us from estimating conductance directly because SMR and Tb were measured at different times (night and morning, respectively). In European kestrels Falco tinnunculus, a food rationing-induced decline in Tb was substantially larger at night than in the morning (Daan et al., 1989), indicating the error introduced when calculating conductance using metabolic rate and Tb measured at different times. Hence, the correlation between BMRm and SMRm is low at least in part due to individual differences in Tb regulation, and individual variation in conductance may have further contributed to this finding, but this remains to be tested.

BMR is often used to characterize energy consumption or minimum cost of self-maintenance of individuals or species. Our finding that BMR and SMR are poorly correlated raises the question whether BMR or SMR is most suitable for this purpose. When individuals are the unit of analysis, for example when relating an individual's minimum energy expenditure to life-history traits (e.g. Burton et al., 2011; Nilsson and Nilsson, 2016), SMR may be preferable because heat loss is an inevitable determinant of an individual's minimum levels of energy expenditure and animals spend much of their time at sub-thermoneutral Ta. Because SMRs at all sub-thermoneutral Ta correlate almost perfectly with each other (Table 2), it appears that individual differences in SMR are equally well characterized at any sub-thermoneutral Ta. However, when species are the unit of analysis, the problem is more intricate because species live at different Ta (Wiersma et al., 2007a,b, 2012). A possible solution might be to quantify SMR a standard number of degrees below the thermoneutral zone. Conversely, at the interspecific level, BMR and SMR may be well correlated, in which case using either BMR or SMR should make little difference for the results, but this needs to be verified.

Acknowledgements

We thank Gerard Overkamp, Jorrick Wubs, Maite Punter, Egbert Koetsier and the late René Beamonte-Barrientos for help with data collection. René, you are dearly missed, both as a colleague and as a friend. We thank Irene Tieleman for the use of the thermocouple and for motivating discussions, and two anonymous reviewers for comments that improved the manuscript.

FOOTNOTES

  • Competing interests

    The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

  • Author contributions

    Conceptualization: M.B., S.V.; Methodology: M.B., S.V.; Software: M.B.; Validation: M.B.; Formal analysis: M.B., S.V.; Investigation: M.B., S.V.; Resources: M.B.; Data curation: M.B., S.V.; Writing - original draft: M.B.; Writing - review & editing: M.B., S.V.; Supervision: S.V.; Project administration: M.B., S.V.; Funding acquisition: S.V.

  • Funding

    The setup of this experiment was funded by Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek Vici grant 865.04.003 to S.V.

  • Supplementary information

    Supplementary information available online at http://jeb.biologists.org/lookup/doi/10.1242/jeb.160069.supplemental

  • Received March 27, 2017.
  • Accepted July 4, 2017.
  • © 2017. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd
http://www.biologists.com/user-licence-1-1/

References

  1. ↵
    1. Angilletta, M. J., Jr.,
    2. Cooper, B. S.,
    3. Schuler, M. S. and
    4. Boyles, J. G.
    (2010). The evolution of thermal physiology in endotherms. Front. Biosci. E2, 861-881. doi:10.2741/e148
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Auer, S. K.,
    2. Bassar, R. D.,
    3. Salin, K. and
    4. Metcalfe, N. B.
    (2016). Repeatability of metabolic rate is lower for animals living under field versus laboratory conditions. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 631-634. doi:10.1242/jeb.133678
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Bates, D.,
    2. Maechler, M.,
    3. Bolker, B. and
    4. Walker, S.
    (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1-48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Bautista, L. M.,
    2. Tinbergen, J.,
    3. Wiersma, P. and
    4. Kacelnik, A.
    (1998). Optimal foraging and beyond: how starlings cope with changes in food availability. Am. Nat. 152, 543-561. doi:10.1086/286189
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    1. Beaulieu, M.
    (2016). A bird in the house: the challenge of being ecologically relevant in captivity. Front. Ecol. Evol. 4, 141. doi:10.3389/fevo.2016.00141
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Biro, P. A. and
    2. Stamps, J. A.
    (2010). Do consistent individual differences in metabolic rate promote consistent individual differences in behavior? Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 653-659. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    1. Boutin, S.
    (1990). Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems, and the future. Can. J. Zool. 68, 203-220. doi:10.1139/z90-031
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  8. ↵
    1. Bouwhuis, S.,
    2. Sheldon, B. C. and
    3. Verhulst, S.
    (2011). Basal metabolic rate and the rate of senescence in the great tit. Funct. Ecol. 25, 829-838. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2011.01850.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  9. ↵
    1. Briga, M. and
    2. Verhulst, S.
    (2015a). What can long-lived mutants tell us about mechanisms causing aging and lifespan variation in natural environments? Exp. Gerontol. 71, 21-26. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2015.09.002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Briga, M. and
    2. Verhulst, S.
    (2015b). Large diurnal temperature range increases bird sensitivity to climate change. Sci. Rep. 5, 16600. doi:10.1038/srep16600
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Briga, M.
    (2016). Growing up and growing old: a longitudinal study on aging in zebra finches. PhD thesis, University of Groningen, The Netherlands.
  12. ↵
    1. Briga, M.,
    2. Koetsier, E.,
    3. Boonekamp, J. J.,
    4. Jimeno, B. and
    5. Verhulst, S.
    (2017). Food availability affects adult survival trajectories depending on early developmental conditions. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 284, 20162287. doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.2287
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Burton, T.,
    2. Killen, S. S.,
    3. Armstrong, J. D. and
    4. Metcalfe, N. B.
    (2011). What causes intraspecific variation in resting metabolic rate and what are its ecological consequences? Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 3465-3473. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1778
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Calder, W. A.
    (1964). Gaseous metabolism and water relations of the zebra finch Taeniopygia castanotis. Physiol. Zool. 37, 400-413. doi:10.1086/physzool.37.4.30152758
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Careau, V.,
    2. Reale, D.,
    3. Garant, D.,
    4. Pelletier, F.,
    5. Speakman, J. R. and
    6. Humphries, M. M.
    (2012). Context-dependent correlation between resting metabolic rate and daily energy expenditure in wild chipmunks. J. Exp. Biol. 418-426. doi:10.1242/jeb.076794
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Careau, V.,
    2. Hoye, B. J.,
    3. O'Dwyer, T. W. and
    4. Buttemer, W. A.
    (2014). Among- and within-individual correlations between basal and maximal metabolic rates in birds. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 3593-3596. doi:10.1242/jeb.108704
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Chappell, M. A. and
    2. Bachman, G. C.
    (1995). Aerobic performance in Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi): variance, ontogeny, and the aerobic capacity model of endothermy. Physiol. Zool. 68, 421-442. doi:10.1086/physzool.68.3.30163777
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Charmantier, A. and
    2. Garant, D.
    (2005). Environmental quality and evolutionary potential: lessons from wild populations. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 272, 1415-1425. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3117
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Cohen, J.
    (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  20. ↵
    1. Daan, S.,
    2. Masman, D.,
    3. Strijkstra, A. and
    4. Verhulst, S.
    (1989). Intraspecific allometry of basal metabolic rate: relations with body size, temperature, composition, and circadian phase in the kestrel, Falco tinnunculus. J. Biol. Rhythms 4, 267-283.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  21. ↵
    1. Daan, S.,
    2. Masman, D. and
    3. Groenwold, A.
    (1990). Avian basal metabolic rates: their association with body composition and energy expenditure in nature. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 259, 333-340.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    1. Day, D. E. and
    2. Bartness, T. J.
    (2001). Effects of foraging effort on body fat and food hoarding in Siberian hamsters. J. Exp. Zool. 289, 162-171. doi:10.1002/1097-010X(20010215)289:3<162::AID-JEZ2>3.0.CO;2-N
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    1. Deerenberg, C.,
    2. Overkamp, G. J. F.,
    3. Visser, G. H. and
    4. Daan, S.
    (1998). Compensation in resting metabolism for experimentally increased activity. J. Comp. Physiol. - B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol. 168, 507-512. doi:10.1007/s003600050171
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Dingemanse, N. J. and
    2. Dochtermann, N. A.
    (2013). Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. J. Anim. Ecol. 82, 39-54. doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Falconer, D. S. and
    2. Mackay, T. F. C.
    (1996). Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Harlow: Longman, .
  26. ↵
    1. Fyhn, M.,
    2. Gabrielsen, G. W.,
    3. Nordøy, E. S.,
    4. Moe, B.,
    5. Langseth, I. and
    6. Bech, C.
    (2001). Individual variation in field metabolic rate of kittiwakes (Rissa tridactyla) during the chick rearing period. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 74, 343-355. doi:10.1086/320419
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. ↵
    1. Geiser, F.
    (2004). Metabolic rate and body temperature reduction during hibernation and daily torpor. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 66, 239-274. doi:10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.032102.115105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  28. ↵
    1. Glazier, D. S.
    (2015). Is metabolic rate a universal “pacemaker” for biological processes? Biol. Rev. 90, 377-407. doi:10.1111/brv.12115
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Griffith, S. C.,
    2. Crino, O. L.,
    3. Andrew, S. C.,
    4. Nomano, F. Y.,
    5. Adkins-Regan, E.,
    6. Alonso-Alvarez, C.,
    7. Bailey, I. E.,
    8. Bittner, S. S.,
    9. Bolton, P. E.,
    10. Boner, W. et al.
    (2017). Variation in reproductive success across captive populations: methodological differences, potential biases and opportunities. Ethology 123, 1-29. doi:10.1111/eth.12576
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. ↵
    1. Hadfield, J. D.
    (2010). MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1-22. doi:10.18637/jss.v033.i02
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    1. Hill, R. W.
    (1972). Determination of oxygen consumption oxygen analyzer by use of the paramagnetic oxygen analyzer. J. Appl. Physiol. 33, 261-263.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    IUPS Thermal Commission (2001). Glossary of terms for thermal physiology, 3rd edn. Jpn. J. Physiol. 51, 245-280.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    1. Koetsier, E.,
    2. Verhulst, S.
    (2011). A simple technique to manipulate foraging costs in seed-eating birds. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 1225-1229. doi:10.1242/jeb.050336
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Körtner, G.,
    2. Brigham, R. M. and
    3. Geiser, F.
    (2000). Winter torpor in a large bird. Nature 407, 318. doi:10.1038/35030297
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Martin, T. E.
    (1987). Food as a limit on breeding: a life-history perspective. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 18, 453-487. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.18.110187.002321
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  36. ↵
    1. McKechnie, A. E.
    (2008). Phenotypic flexibility in basal metabolic rate and the changing view of avian physiological diversity: A review. J. Comp. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol. 178, 235-247. doi:10.1007/s00360-007-0218-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. McKechnie, A. E. and
    2. Lovegrove, B. G.
    (2002). Avian facultative hypothermic responses: a review. Condor 104, 705-724. doi:10.1650/0010-5422(2002)104[0705:AFHRAR]2.0.CO;2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    1. McNab, B. K.
    (1980). On estimating thermal conductance in endotherms. Physiol. Zool. 53, 145-156. doi:10.1086/physzool.53.2.30152577
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. McNab, B. K.
    (1997). On the utility of uniformity in the definition of basal rate of metabolism. Physiol. Zool. 70, 718-720. doi:10.1086/515881
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Meerlo, P.,
    2. Bolle, L.,
    3. Visser, G. H.,
    4. Masman, D. and
    5. Daan, S.
    (1997). Basal metabolic rate in relation to body composition and daily energy expenditure in the field vole, Microtus agrestis. Physiol. Zool. 70, 362-369. doi:10.1086/639616
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Moller, A. P. and
    2. Jennions, M. D.
    (2002). How much variance can be explained by ecologists and evolutionary biologists? Oikos 132, 492-500.
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    1. Nakagawa, S. and
    2. Cuthill, I. C.
    (2007). Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. 82, 591-605. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00027.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Nakagawa, S. and
    2. Schielzeth, H.
    (2010). Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. 85, 935-956. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00141.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Nespolo, R. F. and
    2. Franco, M.
    (2007). Whole-animal metabolic rate is a repeatable trait: a meta-analysis. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 2000-2005. doi:10.1242/jeb.02780
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Nilsson, J. F. and
    2. Nilsson, J.-Å.
    (2016). Fluctuating selection on basal metabolic rate. Ecol. Evol. 6, 1197-1202. doi:10.1002/ece3.1954
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Perrigo, G.
    (1987). Breeding and feeding strategies in deer mice and house mice when females are challenged to work for their food. Anim. Behav. 35, 1298-1316. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80002-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Petit, M.,
    2. Lewden, A. and
    3. Vézina, F.
    (2013). Intra-seasonal flexibility in avian metabolic performance highlights the uncoupling of basal metabolic rate and thermogenic capacity. PLoS ONE 8, e68292. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Prevedello, J. A.,
    2. Dickman, C. R.,
    3. Vieira, M. V. and
    4. Vieira, E. M.
    (2013). Population responses of small mammals to food supply and predators: A global meta-analysis. J. Anim. Ecol. 82, 927-936. doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12072
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. ↵
    1. Robb, G. N.,
    2. McDonald, R. A.,
    3. Chamberlain, D. E. and
    4. Bearhop, S.
    (2008). Food for thought: Supplementary feeding as a driver of ecological change in avian populations. Front. Ecol. Environ. 6, 476-484. doi:10.1890/060152
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  50. ↵
    1. Rønning, B.,
    2. Moe, B. and
    3. Bech, C.
    (2005). Long-term repeatability makes basal metabolic rate a likely heritable trait in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 4663-4669. doi:10.1242/jeb.01941
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Rowiński, P. K. and
    2. Rogell, B.
    (2017). Environmental stress correlates with increases in both genetic and residual variances: a meta-analysis of animal studies. Evolution 71, 1339-1351. doi:10.1111/evo.13201
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  52. ↵
    1. Ruffino, L.,
    2. Salo, P.,
    3. Koivisto, E.,
    4. Banks, P. B. and
    5. Korpimäki, E.
    (2014). Reproductive responses of birds to experimental food supplementation:a meta-analysis. Front. Zool. 11, 80. doi:10.1186/s12983-014-0080-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Scheipl, F.,
    2. Greven, S. and
    3. Küechenhoff, H.
    (2008). Size and power of tests for a zero random effect variance or polynomial regression in additive and linear mixed models. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 52, 3283-3299. doi:10.1016/j.csda.2007.10.022
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  54. ↵
    1. Scholander, P. F.,
    2. Hock, R.,
    3. Walters, V.,
    4. Johnson, F. and
    5. Irving, L.
    (1950). Heat regulation in some arctic and tropical mammals and birds. Biol. Bull. 99, 237-258. doi:10.2307/1538741
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  55. ↵
    1. Schubert, K. A.,
    2. de Vries, G.,
    3. Vaanholt, L. M.,
    4. Meijer, H. A. J.,
    5. Daan, S. and
    6. Verhulst, S.
    (2009). Maternal energy allocation to offspring increases with environmental quality in house mice. Am. Nat. 173, 831-840. doi:10.1086/598495
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  56. ↵
    1. Speakman, J.,
    2. Ergon, T.,
    3. Cavanagh, R.,
    4. Reid, K.,
    5. Scantlebury, D. and
    6. Lambin, X.
    (2003). Resting and daily energy energy expenditures of free-living voles are positively correlared but reflect extrinsic rather than intrinsic effects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 14057-14062. doi:10.1073/pnas.2235671100
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Suarez, R. K. and
    2. Darveau, C. A.
    (2005). Multi-level regulation and metabolic scaling. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 1627-1634. doi:10.1242/jeb.01503
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Swanson, D. L.,
    2. Thomas, N. E.,
    3. Liknes, E. T. and
    4. Cooper, S. J.
    (2012). Intraspecific correlations of basal and maximal metabolic rates in birds and the aerobic capacity model for the evolution of endothermy. PLoS ONE 7, e34271. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034271
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Tieleman, B. I.,
    2. Dijkstra, T. H.,
    3. Klasing, K. C.,
    4. Visser, G. H. and
    5. Williams, J. B.
    (2008). Effects of experimentally increased costs of activity during reproduction on parental investment and self-maintenance in tropical house wrens. Behav. Ecol. 19, 949-959. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn051
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  60. ↵
    1. Vaanholt, L. M.,
    2. De Jong, B.,
    3. Garland, T.,
    4. Daan, S. and
    5. Visser, G. H.
    (2007). Behavioural and physiological responses to increased foraging effort in male mice. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 2013-2024. doi:10.1242/jeb.001974
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Verhulst, S.,
    2. Holveck, M.-J. and
    3. Riebel, K.
    (2006). Long-term effects of manipulated natal brood size on metabolic rate in zebra finches. Biol. Lett. 2, 478-480. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0496
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Versteegh, M. A.,
    2. Helm, B.,
    3. Dingemanse, N. J. and
    4. Tieleman, B. I.
    (2008). Repeatability and individual correlates of basal metabolic rate and total evaporative water loss in birds: a case study in European stonechats. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. - A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 150, 452-457. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2008.05.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  63. ↵
    1. Vézina, F. and
    2. Williams, T. D.
    (2005). The metabolic cost of egg production is repeatable. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 2533-2538. doi:10.1242/jeb.01675
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Vézina, F.,
    2. Jalvingh, K. M.,
    3. Dekinga, A. and
    4. Piersma, T.
    (2006). Acclimation to different thermal conditions in a northerly wintering shorebird is driven by body mass-related changes in organ size. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 3141-3154. doi:10.1242/jeb.02338
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Visscher, P. M.,
    2. Hill, W. G. and
    3. Wray, N. R.
    (2008). Heritability in the genomics era–concepts and misconceptions. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 255-266. doi:10.1038/nrg2322
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  66. ↵
    1. Vuarin, P. and
    2. Henry, P.-Y.
    (2014). Field evidence for a proximate role of food shortage in the regulation of hibernation and daily torpor: a review. J. Comp. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol. 184, 683-697. doi:10.1007/s00360-014-0833-0
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  67. ↵
    1. White, C. R.,
    2. Schimpf, N. G. and
    3. Cassey, P.
    (2013). The repeatability of metabolic rate declines with time. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 1763-1765. doi:10.1242/jeb.076562
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Wiersma, P. and
    2. Tinbergen, J.
    (2003). No nocturnal energetic savings in response to hard work in free-living great tits. Netherlands J. Zool. 52, 263-279. doi:10.1163/156854203764817715
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  69. ↵
    1. Wiersma, P. and
    2. Verhulst, S.
    (2005). Effects of intake rate on energy expenditure, somatic repair and reproduction of zebra finches. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 4091-4098. doi:10.1242/jeb.01854
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Wiersma, P.,
    2. Chappell, M. A. and
    3. Williams, J. B.
    (2007a). Cold- and exercise-induced peak metabolic rates in tropical birds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 20866-20871. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707683104
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. ↵
    1. Wiersma, P.,
    2. Muñoz-Garcia, A.,
    3. Walker, A. and
    4. Williams, J. B.
    (2007b). Tropical birds have a slow pace of life. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 9340-9345. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702212104
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Wiersma, P.,
    2. Nowak, B. and
    3. Williams, J. B.
    (2012). Small organ size contributes to the slow pace of life in tropical birds. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1662-1669. doi:10.1242/jeb.065144
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    1. Zann, R. A.
    (1996). The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  74. ↵
    1. Zhang, Y.,
    2. Eyster, K.,
    3. Liu, J.-S. and
    4. Swanson, D. L.
    (2015). Cross-training in birds: cold and exercise training produce similar changes in maximal metabolic output, muscle masses and myostatin expression in house sparrows, Passer domesticus. J. Exp. Biol. 2190-2200. doi:10.1242/jeb.121822
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • BMR
  • SMR
  • Repeatability
  • Foraging
  • Daily energy expenditure
  • Body temperature

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Individual variation in metabolic reaction norms over ambient temperature causes low correlation between basal and standard metabolic rate
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
Individual variation in metabolic reaction norms over ambient temperature causes low correlation between basal and standard metabolic rate
Michael Briga, Simon Verhulst
Journal of Experimental Biology 2017 220: 3280-3289; doi: 10.1242/jeb.160069
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
Individual variation in metabolic reaction norms over ambient temperature causes low correlation between basal and standard metabolic rate
Michael Briga, Simon Verhulst
Journal of Experimental Biology 2017 220: 3280-3289; doi: 10.1242/jeb.160069

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgements
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Acclimation to warm temperatures has important implications for mitochondrial function in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)
  • Sex-specific microhabitat use is associated with sex-biased thermal physiology in Anolis lizards
  • Early developmental stages of native populations of Ciona intestinalis under increased temperature are affected by local habitat history
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLE

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


The Big Biology podcast

JEB author Christine Cooper talks to Big Biology about her research. In this fascinating JEB sponsored podcast she tells us how tough zebra finches adjust their physiology to cope with extreme climate events. 


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992