Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
Research Article
Costs of colour change in fish: food intake and behavioural decisions
Gwendolen M. Rodgers, Nicholas W. Gladman, Hannah F. Corless, Lesley J. Morrell
Journal of Experimental Biology 2013 216: 2760-2767; doi: 10.1242/jeb.080879
Gwendolen M. Rodgers
1Institute of Integrative and Comparative Biology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: grodgers@mnhn.fr
Nicholas W. Gladman
2Department of Biological Sciences, University of Hull, Hull HU6 7RX, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hannah F. Corless
1Institute of Integrative and Comparative Biology, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lesley J. Morrell
2Department of Biological Sciences, University of Hull, Hull HU6 7RX, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

SUMMARY

Many animals, particularly reptiles, amphibians, fish and cephalopods, have the ability to change their body colour, for functions including thermoregulation, signalling and predator avoidance. Many fish plastically darken their body colouration in response to dark visual backgrounds, and this functions to reduce predation risk. Here, we tested the hypotheses that colour change in fish (1) carries with it an energetic cost and (2) affects subsequent shoal and habitat choice decisions. We demonstrate that guppies (Poecilia reticulata) change colour in response to dark and light visual backgrounds, and that doing so carries an energetic cost in terms of food consumption. By increasing food intake, however, guppies are able to maintain growth rates and meet the energetic costs of changing colour. Following colour change, fish preferentially choose habitats and shoals that match their own body colouration, and maximise crypsis, thus avoiding the need for further colour change but also potentially paying an opportunity cost associated with restriction to particular habitats and social associates. Thus, colour change to match the background is complemented by behavioural strategies, which should act to maximise fitness in variable environments.

INTRODUCTION

Animals such as reptiles, amphibians, fish and cephalopods can change their body colour for functions such as photoprotection, thermoregulation, social signalling and predator avoidance (reviewed in Sugimoto, 2002; Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009; Leclercq et al., 2010). One way in which animals can avoid predators is by altering their colouration to become cryptic against the visual background (Ruxton et al., 2004). Classic examples include the rapid and dynamic background matching of octopuses (Hanlon et al., 1999; Hanlon, 2007; Hanlon et al., 2009) and chameleons (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009). Many fish darken their body colouration in response to dark visual backgrounds (Sugimoto, 2002; Mills and Patterson, 2009; Leclercq et al., 2010), which functions to facilitate predator avoidance and reduce predation risk (Sumner, 1935a; Sumner, 1935b; Whiteley et al., 2011), and is a plastic and reversible change (Sugimoto, 2002; Leclercq et al., 2010).

Body colour darkening in fish is associated with reversible responses of melanophores (Sugimoto, 2002; Mills and Patterson, 2009). Physiological colour change (controlled by the sympathetic nervous system) (Burton, 2008) occurs over short time scales (minutes to days) and involves two hormones: α-melanocyte stimulating hormone (α-MSH) and melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH). α-MSH causes melanosomes to disperse, making the fish appear darker, while MCH causes melanosomes to aggregate, lightening the appearance of the fish (Logan et al., 2006; Mills and Patterson, 2009). Over longer periods (days to weeks) the action of these hormones alters the morphology, density and distribution of melanophores in the dermal cells (Leclercq et al., 2010), known as morphological colour change (Sugimoto, 2002). Here, we will use ‘colour change’ as a general term encompassing either or both physiological and morphological colour change, as we cannot distinguish between the two mechanisms in our study.

The acquisition and expression of colour by animals is known to carry a cost, as pigments must either be obtained through the diet (e.g. carotenoids) (Leclercq et al., 2010) or synthesised by the animal (e.g. melanin) (Mills and Patterson, 2009), and the expression of melanin-based traits is known to be condition dependent in many species [e.g. wasps (Tibbetts and Dale, 2004), butterflies (Talloen et al., 2004) and birds (Piault et al., 2012)] (reviewed in Stoehr, 2006). A significant cost to melanic colour change in fish has yet to be demonstrated (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009), although the suggestion is that there may be non-trivial costs associated with it, similar to those associated with other types of phenotypic plasticity (e.g. Relyea, 2002; Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009).

Behavioural background matching (Garcia and Sih, 2003) is widespread in the animal kingdom, where prey animals select microhabitats that enhance their crypsis to avoid predation. Brown and green morphs of the Pacific tree frog Hyla regilla preferentially select brown and green substrates, respectively (Wente and Phillips, 2003; Wente and Phillips, 2005), and pale and dark morphs of the cichild fish Telmatochromis temporalis defend territories in open (light) or shaded habitats, according to their body colour (Mboko and Kohda, 1995). For shoaling fish, the visual background may consist of other members of the shoal (Endler, 1978), and both western rainbowfish Melanotaenia australis (Rodgers et al., 2010) and the molly Poecilia latipinna (McRobert and Bradner, 1998) preferentially group with similarly coloured group mates, increasing their crypsis against the group and minimising the possibility of being selected by a predator for being phenotypically distinct (the oddity effect) (Landeau and Terborgh, 1986). By selecting habitats or group mates against which they are already colour adapted, fish can simultaneously minimise both the energetic costs of colour change and the risk of predation.

Here, we investigated the energetic costs of colour change and the implications for subsequent behaviour decisions, using Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata Peters 1859) as a model system. Firstly, we document the ability of individual guppies to change colour in response to dark or light visual backgrounds, and explore the time scale on which this occurs. Secondly, we test the hypothesis that there is an energetic cost associated with repeated adaptation to the background, by assessing the food consumption of fish exposed to constantly coloured and changing habitats. Thirdly, we explore whether the energetic cost of repeated colour change has longer-term implications for growth. Finally, we test the prediction that fish select habitats and group mates that match their own colouration, reducing predation risk through behavioural background matching and avoiding the oddity effect, but potentially paying an opportunity cost through restriction to particular habitats and group mates.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study species and housing

We used the descendants of wild-caught (2003–2006) guppies (P. reticulata) that had been held in the aquarium facilities at the Universities of Hull (experiments 1 and 2) and Leeds (experiments 3 and 4) since capture. Hull fish were held in mixed-sex breeding groups in aquaria (200×340×390 mm) containing artificial substrate and plants. All aquaria were on a re-circulating aquarium system at 23–25°C on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle and fed daily on a mix of commercial flake and pellet food. Leeds fish were held in individually filtered aquaria (550×200 mm, filled to a depth of 140 mm) containing gravel to a depth of 1 cm and artificial plastic plants, at a temperature of 25°C, on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle, and were fed daily on commercial flake food. Where appropriate, experimental aquaria (described below) were furnished with artificial substrate (gravel) and/or artificial plants to mimic stock housing conditions. Details of conditions in individual aquaria are below.

Experiment 1: quantification of colour change

Sixty-eight fish, in groups of four consisting of two males and two females of differing body size were selected from the stock tanks. We selected fish of differing body size to allow for individual identification without marking (large female, 19–28.5 mm; small female, 15–22 mm; large male, 17–23.5 mm; small male, 15–20 mm; note that there is overlap between size classes but individuals of the same sex were identifiable in each group). Each group was photographed from above in a shallow dish of water (10 mm depth) using a digital camera (Canon Powershot G12), attached to a copy stand, with the camera positioned 270 mm above the dish. The dish was placed on a background of 1 mm graph paper (to allow for accurate calibration of photographs) with a black and white colour standard. The dish, copy stand and camera were positioned inside a light cube (EZCube 51 cm light tent) and illuminated with broad-spectrum lighting (2×5000 K 30 W ‘Perfect Daylight’ bulbs). No anaesthetic was used to photograph the fish as the procedure took only a few seconds, and anaesthesia is known to affect colouration in fish (Gray et al., 2011).

Following photography, each group of fish was housed in a tank (192×197 mm, filled to a depth of 141 mm) allocated to either ‘black’ or ‘white’ treatments. Black treatment tanks had black side walls, a black base and a black artificial plant. The rear wall of the tank was blue in colour, and the front wall was left uncovered to allow for observation of the fish. White treatment tanks had white side walls, base and plant, and again had a blue rear wall and uncovered front. Tanks were held on a re-circulating aquarium system at 23–25°C on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle. Fish were held in these conditions for 24 h (black, N=12 fish; white, N=12), 48 h (black, N=12; white, N=8) or 2 weeks (black, N=12; white, N=12), and fed daily on a mix of flake and pellet food. Each group was then placed into a small plastic aquarium (197×125×120 mm, filled to a depth of 85 mm) covered on four sides with black or white plastic film to avoid any changes in colouration, and then photographed again following the procedure above. Fish were then returned to the stock tanks. There were no significant differences in body size in fish allocated to the different colour and time treatments (linear model: colour treatment effect, F1,63=1.913, P=0.172; time effect, F1,63=2.198, P=0.143; interaction, F1,62=0.373, P=0.543).

All the digital images (saved as TIFF files) were analysed using ImageJ version 1.45 (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/index.html). Images were scaled using the graph paper included in each photograph and then the body length of each fish was measured in millimetres. The proportion of melanin pigmentation present on the body of the fish was assessed using an adaptation of previous methods (Rodgers et al., 2010). Images were converted to greyscale, then each image was standardised for white balance by selecting five areas in the black and white colour standards included in the photographs, and setting the average value of each of these as the minimum and maximum pixel intensities, using the colour balance options in ImageJ. The outline of each fish was traced to measure the overall area of the visible dorsal surface (excluding the fins). We assessed colouration by setting the pixel threshold intensity to 70, and counted the number of pixels darker than this threshold to give an indication of the overall level of pigmentation. All images of all fish (before and after colour treatment) were analysed in this way. Fish could be individually identified by sex and size to give repeated-measures data. Over the course of the experiment, one fish died in each of the 14 day colour treatments, giving final sample sizes of N=11 fish for these treatments.

To establish whether 24 h was sufficient for colour change to occur when fish were moved from black to white tanks (and vice versa), we first placed mixed-sex groups of four to five fish into either black or white tanks (as above) for 24 h before photographing them and then placing them in tanks of the opposite colour for a further 24 h and photographing them again. Images were taken and analysed using the same methodology. One fish died in the white-to-black treatment, leaving N=13 fish in the black-to-white treatment and N=11 fish in the white-to-black treatment.

Experiment 2: food consumption

Seventy-four adult guppies (a mix of males and females; males, 14.5–21.5 mm, females, 14–26 mm) were removed from the stock tanks and placed in groups of three to five in small (192×197 mm, filled to a depth of 141 mm) aquaria as for experiment 1. Tanks were allocated to black, white and colour change treatments (black, N=25; white, N=24; colour change, N=25 fish). Black treatment tanks had a constant black background and white treatment tanks had a constant white background as in experiment 1. For the colour change treatment, the colour of the side walls, base and plant was changed from black to white (and vice versa) on a daily basis for a period of 9–11 days. To control for the disturbance caused by this colour change treatment, the side walls, base and plant in black and white treatment tanks were removed and replaced on a daily basis. Fish were fed ad libitum daily on small pellet fish food (ZM Systems, Winchester, Hants, UK) after the tanks colours were changed.

Fish were starved for 1 day before feeding trials commenced, to standardise their motivation to feed. Trials took place in a small (192×197×183 mm) tank filled to a depth of 85 mm. Fish were placed individually into the tank and allowed 5 min to acclimatise. Ten individual food pellets (as above) were then added to the tank and a timer was started. If all pellets were eaten, an additional 10 pellets were added immediately following the consumption of the last pellet. The time interval between the consumption of one pellet and the consumption of the next was monitored, and when there was a delay of greater than 1 min, we considered that the fish had reached satiation, and the trial was terminated. The total number of food pellets consumed was then recorded. The sex and body size (measured to the nearest mm using callipers) of the fish was recorded, and individuals were returned to the stock tanks. There was no difference in size between fish allocated to the different treatments for either males (F2,43=0.653, P=0.525) or females (F2,25=0.233, P=0.794), and no fish died during the experiment.

Experiment 3: growth rate

Ninety newborn guppies (0–3 days old) were haphazardly selected from the stock tanks in groups of five. Groups were placed into a shallow dish of water (10 mm) and photographed from above using a Nikon D90 digital camera with a 105 mm Sigma DG macro lens under standardised conditions with daylight spectrum lighting. A scale was included in the image, and images were subsequently used to measure body length. Fish were then introduced to one of three colour treatment tanks in their groups (see below). There was no difference in body length between the fish assigned to the three treatments (linear model: F2,87=1.792, P=0.178).

Treatment tanks were small plastic aquaria (200×140 mm, filled to a depth of 80 mm with aerated water) covered on all four sides with a sleeve of either black or white plastic, which could easily be removed and replaced without disturbing the water. Each tank contained an airstone to aerate and circulate the water, and 20% water changes were carried out on a fortnightly basis, to remove any uneaten food. No artificial plants were used to provide cover for the fish in this experiment, as the fish were very small, young individuals, and there was a risk of harming individuals hiding in the plants when we removed them. Three treatments were used: constant black, constant white and a colour change treatment. Every 2 days, the plastic sleeves were removed and replaced with a sleeve of the same colour for the constant treatments and of the opposite colour for the colour change treatment, thus controlling for disturbance between treatments. Half the tanks in the colour change treatment started with black and half started with white. Treatments were assigned randomly to tanks arranged in a 10×2 array, illuminated evenly from above by a daylight simulation strip light on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle.

Fry were fed daily ad libitum on fry food (ZM Systems; 80–200 μm particle size, 55% protein). Food was weighed to the nearest 0.001 g to ensure all tanks received the same amount of food, and portion size was increased every 2 weeks as the fish grew to ensure ad libitum feeding. After 65 days in the treatments, fish were removed from the tanks, killed with an overdose of MS-222 by a trained technician, weighed to the nearest mg, and photographed to measure length. Death was confirmed by destruction of the brain, according to Schedule 1 of the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, again by a trained technician. Prior to this, mortality over the course of the experiment was 6.7% (6/90 fish died – two in the black treatment, one in the white treatment and three in the colour change treatment). Because of the age of the fish, we were not able to reliably determine sex at the end of the experiment.

Experiment 4: habitat and shoal choice

Sixty-five adult female guppies (19–21 mm) were removed from the stock tanks and housed in groups of five in small plastic aquaria (200×140 mm, filled to a depth of 80 mm), assigned to either ‘black’ or ‘white’ treatments. For each treatment, the exterior walls were covered with either black or white film (as appropriate) and the base was covered with a 10 mm layer of either black or white gravel. Tanks were oxygenated via an airstone, and a full water change of conditioned water was carried out once a week. Tanks were held at 25°C and illuminated by daylight spectrum lighting on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle. Guppies were housed in these tanks for 1–2 weeks before being assessed in the habitat choice experiments, and then returned to the tanks for a further 2–3 weeks before shoal choice experiments were carried out. During this time, fish were fed ad libitum on commercial flake food. There was no difference in body size in fish allocated to the black and white treatments (t-test: t=0.740, d.f.=63, P=0.462). Mortality was not explicitly recorded during this experiment but was very low.

To assess preferences for differently coloured habitats, individual guppies were placed into a habitat choice tank. The choice tank (500×200 mm, filled to a depth of 100 mm with aerated water) was divided into three sections (habitat zones): a black end section (150×200 mm), a white end section (150×200 mm) and a neutral (brown) central section (200×200 mm). Fish could swim freely between the three sections. Each section was coated on the outside with coloured film (black, white or brown as appropriate), and the side facing the observer was left uncovered to allow observations to be made. The base of the tank was covered with a 10 mm depth of black, white or brown gravel. Gravel was contained within a watertight transparent plastic bag to eliminate potentially confounding effects of different chemical cues from the gravel within each habitat (Ward et al., 2004; Ward et al., 2005). Two tanks were used that were mirror images of each other, to remove any potentially confounding effects of side bias.

A single test fish (from either the black or white treatment tanks) was introduced to the test tank and allowed 2 min to acclimatise before observations began. Preliminary observations suggested that this time was sufficient to allow the majority of fish to begin swimming normally and exploring the tank. Any fish that remained motionless or had not swum into all three habitat zones after the 2 min period was excluded from the experiment (two fish were excluded – one from each treatment – giving final sample sizes of N=25 for black treatment fish and N=24 for white treatment fish). Over a 10 min observation period, we recorded the total time spent in each of the three habitat zones. Observations were made from behind a screen to minimise disturbance to the fish. After trials were complete, fish were returned to the black and white treatment tanks from which they had been taken.

Shoaling preferences were assessed using a standard binary choice design (Morrell et al., 2007). The choice tank (600×210 mm, filled to a depth of 100 mm with conditioned water) was covered on three sides with brown cloth and placed on a mid-brown base. The side facing the observer was left uncovered to allow observations to be made. The tank was divided into three sections by transparent, unperforated partitions to allow for transmission of visual but not olfactory cues (which may be associated with recent habitat) (Ward et al., 2004; Ward et al., 2005). Each end (‘stimulus’) compartment measured 210×150 mm, and contained a stimulus shoal of three black or white treatment fish (i.e. black- or white-adapted fish). The 60 mm adjoining each stimulus compartment were defined as ‘shoaling zones’, such that when the test fish entered the zone, it was considered to be shoaling with the stimulus shoal. For each trial, one stimulus compartment contained a shoal of three black treatment fish, and the other contained a shoal of three white treatment fish. The side containing the black treatment fish was randomised between trials to control for any effects of side bias. Stimulus shoals were selected from different colour tanks to the test fish to avoid any confounding effects of familiarity (Griffiths and Magurran, 1997). Stimulus shoals were allowed 2 min to settle before the introduction of the test fish. Observations suggested this was sufficient for the shoals to begin swimming normally.

A single test fish was then introduced to the central compartment, and allowed 2 min to acclimatise before a 10 min observation period began. During the acclimatisation period, all test fish (N=20 for black treatment fish, N=17 for white treatment fish) began swimming and entered both shoaling zones, and thus no fish were excluded on the basis of inactivity. We recorded the cumulative time spent in each shoaling zone, which was then expressed as a proportion of the total shoaling time (i.e. the sum of the time spent in the two shoaling zones). After the trial was completed, test fish were returned to their colour tanks, and subsequently used as stimulus fish, but no fish that had been used as a stimulus fish was subsequently used as a test fish.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out using R version 2.13.0 (R Development Core Team, 2011). Visual inspection of residual and normal quantile–quantile plots was used to assess normality of data. Appropriate transformations or non-parametric tests were used where the assumption of normality was not supported.

Experiment 1

The proportion of black body colouration was arcsin transformed prior to analysis to meet assumptions of normality. We assessed the effect of stage (before or after exposure to the colour tanks) and sex, and their interaction on the proportion of black colouration for each time period (24 h, 48 h and 2 weeks) and colour treatment individually, using linear mixed effects models (Bates et al., 2011). Fish ID was included in the model as a random effect to account for the repeated-measures nature of the data. General linear models were used to confirm that there was no difference in the starting colour of fish allocated to the different colour or time treatments. There was no significant interaction and no significant effect of sex for any of the analyses, and so only the results of models including stage (before or after exposure to the colour tanks) are reported here.

Experiment 2

We assessed the effect of colour treatment (black, white or changing) and sex, and their interaction on the total number of food pellets consumed using a linear mixed effects model with body size as a random factor. The number of pellets consumed was log transformed to meet the assumptions of a normal error structure.

Experiment 3

Length and mass data conformed to the assumptions of normality, and so we used general linear mixed effects models to assess the effect of colour treatment on the final body length and mass of the fish, with tank ID as a random effect to control for potential non-independence of fish housed within the same tank.

Experiment 4

To test for an effect of colour treatment on habitat preference, and to account for the fact that the proportion of time spent in each colour zone was not independent of the time in the other zones, we used a series of one-sample Wilcoxon tests combined with a correction for multiple testing [false discovery rate (FDR) control] (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Data were split on the basis of fish colour and the proportion of time spent in each zone was tested against an expectation based on the size of the zone (expected proportions: black, 0.3; white, 0.3; neutral, 0.4). FDR control was applied against the three tests for each colour of fish. To test whether black treatment and white treatment fish showed a significant preference for shoaling with the colour-matched shoal over the unmatched shoal, we used non-parametric one-sample Wilcoxon tests, as the data were non-normal and could not be satisfactorily transformed to meet the assumptions of normality. We compared the proportion of time spent shoaling with the matched shoal against median value of 0.5, representing random association patterns (i.e. no preference for either shoal colour), for black and white treatment fish separately.

RESULTS

Experiment 1: quantification of colour change

Fish showed a significant darkening of body colouration after 48 h and 2 weeks in black treatment tanks (Table 1, Fig. 1A) but this was not evident after 24 h (Table 1). Fish in white treatment tanks showed a significant lightening of body colouration after all time periods (Table 1, Fig. 1A). When moved from a black to a white tank, fish showed a significant lightening in colour (Table 1, Fig. 1B), while fish moved from a white to a black tank showed a significant darkening (Table 1, Fig. 1B).

Experiment 2: food consumption

There was no significant interaction between colour treatment and sex (F2,51=2.319, P=0.108), so this was removed from the model and only the main effects are presented here. There was a significant effect of colour treatment and sex (but no interaction) on the number of food pellets consumed (treatment, F2,53=13.288, P<0.0001; sex, F1,53=10.767, P=0.0018; Fig. 2). Fish in the colour change treatment consumed more pellets than fish in the static black and white treatments (Table 2). Male fish also consumed more than female fish across all treatments (Table 2, Fig. 2).

Experiment 3: growth rate

There was no significant effect of colour treatment (black, white or colour change) on the final length (linear mixed effects model: F=1.494, P=0.256, N=84 observations in 18 groups) or mass (linear mixed effects model: F=1.031, P=0.381, N=84 observations in 18 groups) of fish.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Quantification of colour change

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

(A) Change in the proportion of black body colouration for fish exposed to black (filled bars) and white (open bars) treatments for 24 h, 48 h and 14 days. (B) Change in the proportion of black body colouration for fish moved from black to white treatments (and vice versa) for 24 h. Error bars represent ±1 s.e.m. Asterisks indicate a significant change in colouration (see Table 1).

Experiment 4: habitat and shoal choice

Fish from the black treatment spent significantly more time in the black zone (V=262, adjusted P=0.012) and significantly less time in the white zone (V=7, adjusted P<0.001) than expected by chance (Fig. 3A). The proportion of time spent in the neutral zone did not differ from random expectation (V=172, adjusted P=0.809). Fish from the white treatment spent significantly more time in the neutral zone (V=260, adjusted P=0.002), and significantly less time in the black (V=69, P=0.031) and white (V=78, P=0.040) zones than expected (Fig. 3A). Both black and white treatment fish showed a significant preference for the colour-matched shoal (Wilcoxon signed rank test; black treatment fish: V=170, d.f.=19, P=0.016; white treatment fish: V=131, d.f.=16, P=0.011; Fig. 3B).

DISCUSSION

Here, we have demonstrated that there may be costs associated with colour change in guppies. Fish exposed to a changing environment changed their body colouration and increased their food consumption relative to those exposed to a constantly coloured environment. A cost to colour change in fish has been predicted (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009) but not previously demonstrated. However, we also found that when food was freely available, fish were able to meet this potential cost, and did not suffer negative consequences in terms of growth rate. It is well known that poor conditions, particularly food availability, can cause slower growth rates in fish (Metcalfe and Monaghan, 2001; Lee et al., 2012), and that improved food availability can lead to compensatory (or catch-up) growth (Ali et al., 2003; Hector and Nakagawa, 2012). Thus, we would predict that if food supplies were limited, the costs associated with colour change that we identified in experiment 2 could lead to a reduction in growth rate, but this is yet to be tested. Further work is needed to conclusively demonstrate an energetic cost associated with colour change, which may be linked to the mechanisms of colour change (physiological versus morphological).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Food consumption

In our experiments, fish were moved from the neutrally coloured stock tanks into the treatment tanks, and so those fish exposed to the constant black and white treatments would also have undergone some colour change, becoming darker or paler. In contrast to the fish in the colour change treatments, however, this change would have slowed and stabilised over time (e.g. Péan, 2012), while fish in the colour change treatment would have repeatedly undergone the initial rapid colour change. The large increase in food intake by fish in the colour change treatment strongly suggests that the initial rapid colour change has large associated energetic costs.

Our finding that males had an elevated food intake relative to females is unsurprising: sex differences in behaviour have been well documented in guppies (Houde, 1997; Magurran and Garcia, 2000; Magurran, 2005). Males are generally more active (Reader and Laland, 2000) and less risk averse (Magurran and Seghers, 1994) than females, and move between shoals more than females (Croft et al., 2003), as males attempt to maximise mate encounters and increase reproductive success. The increased activity levels of males provide a simple explanation for the increased food intake we observed.

It is possible that the increased food intake by fish from the colour change treatment could have been due to their being less risk averse. Juvenile cod (Gadus morhua) and guppies that experience unpredictable environments during early life are known to become ‘bolder’ (Braithwaite and Salvanes, 2005; Chapman et al., 2010), leaving a refuge more quickly and being more explorative in a novel area, and this may cause them to forage more rapidly once introduced to the test tank. We did not specifically record latency to feed in the behavioural trials, but all fish were swimming normally by the time the first food pellet was added, and the majority of fish immediately attacked the first pellet, suggesting that differences in boldness due to recent experience may not have been important in determining the likelihood of foraging. Female fish did appear to take longer to settle during the 5 min acclimatisation period than males (N.W.G., personal observation), but we noted no differences in acclimatisation behaviour between colour treatments, and all fish experienced equal disturbance (changing of backgrounds) in a predictable manner during the short experimental period.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Number of food pellets consumed by female (filled bars) and male (open bars) guppies from the three colour treatments. Error bars represent ±1 s.e.m.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

(A) The proportion of time spent by black treatment and white treatment fish in each of the three habitat zones: black habitat (dark filled bars), neutral habitat (light filled bars) and white habitat (open bars). Asterisks indicate significant deviation from random expectation, indicated by the dashed line. (B) Mean proportion of time spent shoaling with the colour-matched shoal by black (filled bars) and white (open bars) treatment fish. The horizontal dashed line indicates no preference; asterisks denote a significant deviation from this. Error bars are ±1 s.e.m.

We also demonstrated that fish preferred both habitats and shoals that matched their own colouration: black-adapted fish showed a significant preference for the black-adapted shoal over the white-adapted one, and for darker habitats, and avoided white habitats. White-adapted fish also preferred colour-matched shoals, and neutrally coloured habitats, avoiding both black and white habitats. White habitats may be unattractive to fish for a range of reasons. The lighter background, and more reflected light, may mean that it is easier for a predator to identify a moving or camouflaged prey item (e.g. Strand et al., 2007). In guppies, the highest predation risk generally occurs at maximum light levels (Endler, 1987), and in salamanders (Ambystoma barbouri and A. texanum), the presence of olfactory cues from predatory fish resulted in a preference for dark substrates over light ones (Garcia and Sih, 2003). There is also evidence that living in light-coloured habitats may have other costly implications for fish: growth and survival in juvenile yellow seahorses (Hippocampus kuda), for example, were significantly reduced in light habitats compared with dark ones (Pawar et al., 2011).

Many animals are known to select habitats that best match their colouration (known as ‘behavioural background matching’) in order to increase crypsis and reduce predation (Garcia and Sih, 2003; Ruxton et al., 2004). Both black- and white-adapted fish avoided habitats of the opposite colour, where they would be most conspicuous, preferring instead those where they were more cryptic. White-adapted fish were not white in colour, and may have been more cryptic against the neutral background than against the white background, explaining their preference for the neutral background. The choice of neutral habitats by white-adapted fish also provides evidence that the preferences we observed were not based only on familiarity with the habitat (Stamps and Swaisgood, 2007). White-adapted fish preferred neutral habitats to white ones, but a preference for white would be predicted if familiarity with recent habitat drove their decision making. Thus, we conclude that both black- and white-adapted fish are selecting habitats that maximise crypsis. Similarly, white morphs of black-and-white mollies (Poecilia latipinna) show no preference for white habitats over black ones, in contrast to their black counterparts, who prefer black backgrounds (Bradner and McRobert, 2001), suggesting that white habitats are not preferred even for those animals that would be most cryptic against them. ‘Matching habitat choice’ carries with it implications for gene flow and can promote population differentiation and adaptation (Edelaar et al., 2008), as individuals actively choose microhabitats that maximise fitness (Karpestam et al., 2011).

Both black- and white-adapted fish preferentially associated with colour-matched shoals. It is well established that fish assort into shoals based on phenotype: there is evidence for assortment by species (Keenleyside, 1955), body size (Krause et al., 1998; Ward and Krause, 2001) and colour (McRobert and Bradner, 1998; Rodgers et al., 2010). By shoaling with phenotypically matched fish (those that are similar in appearance), individuals can benefit from a reduced risk of predation via two interlinked mechanisms: the confusion (Krakauer, 1995) and oddity (Ohguchi, 1978; Landeau and Terborgh, 1986) effects. The oddity effect allows a predator to overcome the confusion caused by a moving group of phenotypically similar individuals (the confusion effect) by selecting one that is distinct (‘odd’) as the target, and together these mechanisms should lead to the evolution of phenotype-assorted groups.

The ability to associate with colour-matched fish and choose habitats that maximise crypsis suggests that a fish is able to judge its own body colouration. A fish may do this either through a self-referent matching process or by assessing the colour of those with whom it has recent experience and making decisions accordingly. While many fish learn their phenotype when young (Engeszer et al., 2004), there is evidence that recent experience can also contribute to this assessment (Mateo, 2004; Witte, 2006; Gómez-Laplaza, 2009). In sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), self-referent phenotype matching is used in social decisions, and this, in common with much social behaviour in fish, is mediated by chemical rather than visual cues (Ward et al., 2005; Ward and Currie, 2013). Familiarity (associating with individuals of shared recent experience) is a key factor structuring fish shoals, supporting the notion that fish benefit from association with those that have experienced a similar recent environment (Ward and Hart, 2003).

By selecting habitats or shoals that match their current colouration, fish reduce their risk of predation via crypsis against the background (Ruxton et al., 2004) or shoal (Endler, 1978). Even though colour adjustment could be made in a matter of minutes in some species, it is never instant (Leclercq et al., 2010), and will be associated with an increased risk of predation before adaptation to the background is complete. Selecting matching habitats and shoals removes the need to adjust body colouration and pay the associated cost, but simultaneously may represent an opportunity cost (Ruxton et al., 2004) as it may act to restrict individuals to particular habitats or associates. Herbert and Emery suggest that the high cost associated with melanin synthesis restricts melanic morphs to areas of high UV radiation, for example (Herbert and Emery, 1990). All prey animals at risk from visual predators will face an opportunity cost while they remain in microhabitats with backgrounds that maximise their camouflage (Ruxton et al., 2004). This cost is reduced by the ability to change colour to match different backgrounds, and is thus inversely related to speed of colour change.

Colour change for background matching may have implications for other behaviours. Colour is widely used as a signal (Endler, 1992; Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003): melanin, for example, is linked to social signalling, particularly in advertising social status [e.g. house sparrows Passer domesticus (Møller, 1987), Atlantic salmon Salmo salar (O'Connor et al., 1999) and arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus (Höglund et al., 2002)]. Thus, there is a potential trade-off between colour change for background adaptation and colour change associated with other functions: colour change that is beneficial in the context of background matching may be detrimental in the context of social signalling, for example (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009). Colour change for background matching could also affect colour patterns used in sexual signals, masking the intensity of colour patches, or minimising variation between males, for example. The interaction between colour change for crypsis and colour change for thermoregulation presents another potential conflict for many species including reptiles and amphibians (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009), though this is likely to be of little importance to exclusively aquatic organisms.

Prey animals often have a repertoire of potential responses to increased predation risk, ranging from immediate behavioural responses to plastic morphological change to shifts in life history strategies (Garcia and Sih, 2003). We show here that colour change to match the visual background is complemented by behavioural strategies that minimise the need to pay the cost associated with colour change, but which may also represent an opportunity cost to the individual. Together, these strategies should act to maximise fitness in variable environments, but the extent to which these costs and decisions impact on fitness in wild populations is yet to be examined, and we know little about how colour change for crypsis interacts with other functions of colour (Stuart-Fox and Moussalli, 2009).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Neil Crossley for technical assistance with the growth rate experiments. Scott Fawcett, Neil Crossley and the CBS staff at the University of Leeds and Alan Smith at the University of Hull provided technical support in the aquaria. Pippa Hamshaw assisted with pilot trials for the growth rate experiments. We also thank Steve Sait for useful discussions and two anonymous referees for comments on the manuscript.

FOOTNOTES

  • AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

    G.M.R. and L.J.M. conceived and designed the experiments. All authors collected and analysed the data. G.M.R. and L.J.M. interpreted the findings. All authors drafted and revised the article.

  • COMPETING INTERESTS

    No competing interests declared.

  • FUNDING

    G.M.R. was supported by a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) doctoral training grant.

  • © 2013. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Ali, M.,
    2. Nicieza, A. and
    3. Wootton, R.
    (2003). Compensatory growth in fishes: a response to growth depression. Fish Fish. 1, 147-190.
    OpenUrl
  2. ↵
    1. Bates, D.,
    2. Maechler, M. and
    3. Bolker, B.
    (2011). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-39. Available at: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html.
  3. ↵
    1. Benjamini, Y. and
    2. Hochberg, Y.
    (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 1, 289-300.
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. Bradner, J. and
    2. McRobert, S.
    (2001). Background colouration influences body colour segregation in mollies. J. Fish Biol. 1, 673-681.
    OpenUrl
  5. ↵
    1. Braithwaite, V. A. and
    2. Salvanes, A. G.
    (2005). Environmental variability in the early rearing environment generates behaviourally flexible cod: implications for rehabilitating wild populations. Proc. Biol. Sci. 1, 1107-1113.
    OpenUrl
  6. ↵
    1. Burton, D.
    (2008). A physiological interpretation of pattern changes in a flatfish. J. Fish Biol. 1, 639-649.
    OpenUrl
  7. ↵
    1. Chapman, B.B.,
    2. Morrell, L. J. and
    3. Krause, J.
    (2010). Unpredictability in food supply during early life influences boldness in fish. Behav. Ecol. 1, 501-506.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. Croft, D.,
    2. Arrowsmith, B.,
    3. Bielby, J.,
    4. Skinner, K.,
    5. White, E.,
    6. Couzin, I.,
    7. Magurran, A. E.,
    8. Ramnarine, I. and
    9. Krause, J.
    (2003). Mechanisms underlying shoal composition in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Oikos 1, 429-438.
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. Edelaar, P.,
    2. Siepielski, A. M. and
    3. Clobert, J.
    (2008). Matching habitat choice causes directed gene flow: a neglected dimension in evolution and ecology. Evolution 1, 2462-2472.
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    1. Endler, J. A.
    (1978). A predator's view of animal color patterns. Evol. Biol. 1, 319-364.
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Endler, J.
    (1987). Predation, light intensity and courtship behaviour in Poecilia reticulata (Pisces, Poeciliidae). Anim. Behav. 1, 1376-1385.
    OpenUrl
  12. ↵
    1. Endler, J. A.
    (1992). Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am. Nat. 139, S125-S153.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Engeszer, R. E.,
    2. Ryan, M. J. and
    3. Parichy, D. M.
    (2004). Learned social preference in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 1, 881-884.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Garcia, T. S. and
    2. Sih, A.
    (2003). Color change and color-dependent behavior in response to predation risk in the salamander sister species Ambystoma barbouri and Ambystoma texanum. Oecologia 1, 131-139.
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    1. Gómez-Laplaza, L. M.
    (2009). Recent social environment affects colour-assortative shoaling in juvenile angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare). Behav. Processes 1, 39-44.
    OpenUrl
  16. ↵
    1. Gray, S. M. F.,
    2. Hart, F. L.,
    3. Tremblay, M. E. M.,
    4. Lisney, T. J. and
    5. Hawryshyn, C. W.
    (2011). The effects of handling time, ambient light, and anaesthetic method, on the standardized measurement of fish colouration. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 1, 330-342.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    1. Griffiths, S. and
    2. Magurran, A. E.
    (1997). Familiarity in schooling fish: How long does it take to acquire? Anim. Behav. 1, 945-949.
    OpenUrl
  18. ↵
    1. Hanlon, R.
    (2007). Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Curr. Biol. 17, R400-R404.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Hanlon, R. T.,
    2. Forsythe, J. W. and
    3. Joneschild, D. E.
    (1999). Crypsis, conspicuousness, mimicry and polyphenism as antipredator defences of foraging octopuses on Indo-Pacific coral reefs, with a method of quantifying crypsis from video tapes. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 1, 1-22.
    OpenUrl
  20. ↵
    1. Hanlon, R. T.,
    2. Chiao, C.-C.,
    3. Mäthger, L. M.,
    4. Barbosa, A.,
    5. Buresch, K. C. and
    6. Chubb, C.
    (2009). Cephalopod dynamic camouflage: bridging the continuum between background matching and disruptive coloration. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 1, 429-437.
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    1. Hector, K. L. and
    2. Nakagawa, S.
    (2012). Quantitative analysis of compensatory and catch-up growth in diverse taxa. J. Anim. Ecol. 1, 583-593.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    1. Herbert, P. and
    2. Emery, C.
    (1990). The adaptive significance of cuticular pigmentation in Daphnia. Funct. Ecol. 1, 703-710.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    1. Höglund, E.,
    2. Balm, P. H. and
    3. Winberg, S.
    (2002). Behavioural and neuroendocrine effects of environmental background colour and social interaction in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus). J. Exp. Biol. 1, 2535-2543.
    OpenUrl
  24. ↵
    1. Houde, A. E.
    (1997). Sex, Color, and Mate Choice in Guppies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
    1. Karpestam, E.,
    2. Wennersten, L. and
    3. Forsman, A.
    (2012). Matching habitat choice by experimentally mismatched phenotypes. Evol. Ecol. 1, 893-907.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Keenleyside, M. H. A.
    (1955). Some aspects of the schooling behaviour of fish. Behaviour 1, 183-248.
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    1. Krakauer, D.
    (1995). Groups confuse predators by exploiting perceptual bottlenecks: a connectionist model of the confusion effect. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1, 421-429.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    1. Krause, J.,
    2. Godin, J. and
    3. Rubenstein, D.
    (1998). Group choice as a function of group size differences and assessment time in fish: the influence of species vulnerability to predation. Ethology 1, 68-74.
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    1. Landeau, L. and
    2. Terborgh, J.
    (1986). Oddity and the confusion effect in predation. Anim. Behav. 1, 1372-1380.
    OpenUrl
  29. ↵
    1. Leclercq, E.,
    2. Taylor, J. F. and
    3. Migaud, H.
    (2010). Morphological skin colour changes in teleosts. Fish Fish. 1, 159-193.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    1. Lee, W.-S.,
    2. Monaghan, P. and
    3. Metcalfe, N. B.
    (2012). The pattern of early growth trajectories affects adult breeding performance. Ecology 1, 902-912.
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    1. Logan, D. W.,
    2. Burn, S. F. and
    3. Jackson, I. J.
    (2006). Regulation of pigmentation in zebrafish melanophores. Pigment Cell Res. 1, 206-213.
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    1. Magurran, A. E.
    (2005). Evolutionary Ecology: The Trinidadian Guppy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  33. ↵
    1. Magurran, A. E. and
    2. Garcia, C. M.
    (2000). Sex differences in behaviour as an indirect consequence of mating system. J. Fish Biol. 1, 839-857.
    OpenUrl
  34. ↵
    1. Magurran, A. E. and
    2. Seghers, B.
    (1994). Sexual conflict as a consequence of ecology – evidence from Guppy, Poecilia reticulata, populations in Trinidad. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1, 31-36.
    OpenUrl
  35. ↵
    1. Mateo, J. M.
    (2004). Recognition systems and biological organization: the perception component of social recognition. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 1, 729-745.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    1. Maynard Smith, J. and
    2. Harper, D.
    (2003). Animal Signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  37. ↵
    1. Mboko, S. K. and
    2. Kohda, M.
    (1995). Pale and dark dichromatism related to microhabitats in a herbivorous Tanganyikan cichlid fish, Telmatochromis temporalis. J. Ethol. 1, 77-83.
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    1. McRobert, S. P. and
    2. Bradner, J.
    (1998). The influence of body coloration on shoaling preferences in fish. Anim. Behav. 1, 611-615.
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Metcalfe, N. B. and
    2. Monaghan, P.
    (2001). Compensation for a bad start: grow now, pay later? Trends Ecol. Evol. 1, 254-260.
    OpenUrl
  40. ↵
    1. Mills, M. G. and
    2. Patterson, L. B.
    (2009). Not just black and white: pigment pattern development and evolution in vertebrates. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 1, 72-81.
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    1. Møller, A. P.
    (1987). Variation in badge size in male house sparrows Passer domesticus: evidence for status signalling. Anim. Behav. 1, 1637-1644.
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    1. Morrell, L. J.,
    2. Hunt, K.,
    3. Croft, D. and
    4. Krause, J.
    (2007). Diet, familiarity and shoaling decisions in guppies. Anim. Behav. 1, 311-319.
    OpenUrl
  43. ↵
    1. O'Connor, K. I.,
    2. Metcalfe, N. B. and
    3. Taylor, A. C.
    (1999). Does darkening signal submission in territorial contests between juvenile Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar? Anim. Behav. 1, 1269-1276.
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    1. Ohguchi, O.
    (1978). Experiments on the selection against colour oddity of water fleas by three-spined sticklebacks. Z. Tierpsychol. 1, 254-267.
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    1. Pawar, H. B.,
    2. Sanaye, S. V.,
    3. Murugan, A.,
    4. Sreepada and
    5. Tanu, R. A.
    . 2011. Effect of background color of tanks on growth and survival of juvenile yellow seahorse, Hippocampus kuda (Bleeker 1852), in the Pelagic Phase. Israeli Journal of Aquaculture 63, IIC:63.2011.621.
  46. ↵
    1. Péan, S.
    (2012). Effects of Persistent Organic Pollutants on Fish Behaviour. PhD thesis, Universite de La Rochelle.
  47. ↵
    1. Piault, R.,
    2. van den Brink, V. and
    3. Roulin, A.
    (2012). Condition-dependent expression of melanin-based coloration in the Eurasian kestrel. Naturwissenschaften 1, 391-396.
    OpenUrl
  48. ↵
    1. R Development Core Team
    (2011). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/.
  49. ↵
    1. Reader, S. M. and
    2. Laland, K. N.
    (2000). Diffusion of foraging innovations in the guppy. Anim. Behav. 1, 175-180.
    OpenUrl
  50. ↵
    1. Relyea, R. A.
    (2002). Costs of phenotypic plasticity. Am. Nat. 1, 272-282.
    OpenUrl
  51. ↵
    1. Rodgers, G. M.,
    2. Kelley, J. L. and
    3. Morrell, L. J.
    (2010). Colour change and assortment in the western rainbowfish. Anim. Behav. 1, 1025-1030.
    OpenUrl
  52. ↵
    1. Ruxton, G.,
    2. Sherratt, T. and
    3. Speed, M.
    2004. Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  53. ↵
    1. Stamps, J. A. and
    2. Swaisgood, R. R.
    (2007). Someplace like home: experience, habitat selection and conservation biology. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1, 392-409.
    OpenUrl
  54. ↵
    1. Stoehr, A. M.
    (2006). Costly melanin ornaments: the importance of taxon? Funct. Ecol. 1, 276-281.
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    1. Strand, Å.,
    2. Alanärä, A.,
    3. Staffan, F. and
    4. Magnhagen, C.
    (2007). Effects of tank colour and light intensity on feed intake, growth rate and energy expenditure of juvenile Eurasian perch, Perca fluviatilis L. Aquaculture 1, 312-318.
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    1. Stuart-Fox, D. and
    2. Moussalli, A.
    (2009). Camouflage, communication and thermoregulation: lessons from colour changing organisms. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B 1, 463-470.
    OpenUrl
  57. ↵
    1. Sugimoto, M.
    (2002). Morphological color changes in fish: regulation of pigment cell density and morphology. Microsc. Res. Tech. 1, 496-503.
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    1. Sumner, F. B.
    (1935a). Evidence for the protective value of changeable coloration in fishes. Am. Nat. 1, 245-266.
    OpenUrl
  59. ↵
    1. Sumner, F. B.
    (1935b). Studies of protective color change: III. experiments with fishes both as predators and prey. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1, 345-353.
    OpenUrl
  60. ↵
    1. Talloen, W.,
    2. Van Dyck, H. and
    3. Lens, L.
    (2004). The cost of melanization: butterfly wing coloration under environmental stress. Evolution 1, 360-366.
    OpenUrl
  61. ↵
    1. Tibbetts, E. A. and
    2. Dale, J.
    (2004). A socially enforced signal of quality in a paper wasp. Nature 1, 218-222.
    OpenUrl
  62. ↵
    1. Ward, A. J. W. and
    2. Currie, S.
    (2013). Shoaling fish can size-assort by chemical cues alone. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1, 667-673.
    OpenUrl
  63. ↵
    1. Ward, A. J. W. and
    2. Hart, P. J. B.
    (2003). The effects of kin and familiarity on interactions between fish. Fish Fish. 1, 348-358.
    OpenUrl
  64. ↵
    1. Ward, A. and
    2. Krause, J.
    (2001). Body length assortative shoaling in the European minnow, Phoxinus phoxinus. Anim. Behav. 1, 617-621.
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    1. Ward, A.,
    2. Hart, P. and
    3. Krause, J.
    (2004). The effects of habitat- and diet-based cues on association preferences in three-spined sticklebacks. Behav. Ecol. 1, 925-929.
    OpenUrl
  66. ↵
    1. Ward, A.,
    2. Holbrook, R.,
    3. Krause, J. and
    4. Hart, P.
    (2005). Social recognition in sticklebacks: the role of direct experience and habitat cues. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1, 575-583.
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    1. Wente, W. H. and
    2. Phillips, J. B.
    (2003). Fixed green and brown color morphs and a novel color-changing morph of the Pacific tree frog Hyla regilla. Am. Nat. 1, 461-473.
    OpenUrl
  68. ↵
    1. Wente, W. H. and
    2. Phillips, J. B.
    (2005). Microhabitat selection by the Pacific treefrog, Hyla regilla. Anim. Behav. 1, 279-287.
    OpenUrl
  69. ↵
    1. Whiteley, A. R.,
    2. Bergstrom, C. A.,
    3. Linderoth, T. and
    4. Tallmon, D. A.
    (2011). The spectre of past spectral conditions: colour plasticity, crypsis and predation risk in freshwater sculpin from newly deglaciated streams. Ecol. Freshwat. Fish 1, 80-91.
    OpenUrl
  70. ↵
    1. Witte, K.
    2006. Learning and mate choice. In Fish Cognition and Behavior (ed. Brown, C., Laland, K. and Krause, J.), pp. 70-95. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

Keywords

  • physiological colour change
  • morphological colour change
  • Phenotypic plasticity
  • Camouflage
  • Background matching

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Costs of colour change in fish: food intake and behavioural decisions
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
Costs of colour change in fish: food intake and behavioural decisions
Gwendolen M. Rodgers, Nicholas W. Gladman, Hannah F. Corless, Lesley J. Morrell
Journal of Experimental Biology 2013 216: 2760-2767; doi: 10.1242/jeb.080879
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
Costs of colour change in fish: food intake and behavioural decisions
Gwendolen M. Rodgers, Nicholas W. Gladman, Hannah F. Corless, Lesley J. Morrell
Journal of Experimental Biology 2013 216: 2760-2767; doi: 10.1242/jeb.080879

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • FOOTNOTES
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Nest substrate and tool shape significantly affect the mechanics and energy requirements of avian eggshell puncture
  • Jaw kinematics and tongue protraction-retraction during Chewing and drinking in the pig
  • Early developmental stages of native populations of Ciona intestinalis under increased temperature are affected by local habitat history
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLES

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Predicting the Future: Species Survival in a Changing World

Read our new special issue exploring the significant role of experimental biology in assessing and predicting the susceptibility or resilience of species to future, human-induced environmental change.


Adam Hardy wins the 2020 Journal of Experimental Biology Outstanding Paper Prize

Congratulations to winner Adam Hardy for his work showing that goby fins are as touch sensitive as primate fingertips. Read Adam’s paper and find out more about the 12 papers nominated for the award.


Stark trade-offs and elegant solutions in arthropod visual systems

Many elegant eye specializations that evolved in response to visual challenges continue to be discovered. A new Review by Meece et al. summarises exciting solutions evolved by insects and other arthropods in response to specific visual challenges.


Head bobbing gives pigeons a sense of perspective

Pigeons might look goofy with their head-bobbing walk, but it turns out that the ungainly head manoeuvre allows the birds to judge distance.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992