Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
    • For library administrators
Research Article
Parental behavior in pythons is responsive to both the hydric and thermal dynamics of the nest
Zachary Stahlschmidt, Dale F. DeNardo
Journal of Experimental Biology 2010 213: 1691-1696; doi: 10.1242/jeb.041095
Zachary Stahlschmidt
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: zs@asu.edu
Dale F. DeNardo
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

SUMMARY

Parental behavior contributes to the success of a diverse array of taxa, and female-only nest attendance is particularly widespread. Python egg-brooding behavior is an intriguing example of female-only nest attendance because it significantly influences several critical developmental variables, namely embryonic predation, hydration, respiration and temperature. During brooding, females predominately adopt a tightly coiled posture that reduces the exchange of heat, water vapor, O2 and CO2 between the nest and clutch environment, which benefits egg water balance at the cost of respiration. To determine the plasticity of this important behavior, we manipulated nest temperature and humidity while monitoring nest–clutch thermal, hydric and respiratory relationships to test the hypothesis that female Children's pythons (Antaresia childreni) modify their egg-brooding behavior due to an interaction between environmental thermal and hydric conditions. During moderate and high nest humidity treatments (23 and 32 g m−3 H2O, respectively), females spent more time coiling tightly when the nest was cooling than when it was warming, which benefited clutch temperature. However, brooding females in low-humidity nest environments (13 g m−3 H2O) showed a high frequency of tight coiling even when the nest was warming; thus, nest temperature and humidity had an interactive effect on egg-brooding behavior in support of our hypothesis. Our results also suggest that certain egg-brooding behaviors (i.e. postural adjustments) are more energetically costly to females than other behaviors (i.e. tight coiling). In sum, we provide empirical support for the adaptive plasticity of python egg-brooding behavior, which offers insight into the general significance of female-only nest attendance in animals.

INTRODUCTION

Parental care is instrumental to the success of a diverse array of taxa, and its broad evolutionary significance has been the motivation for substantial investigation (Williams, 1966; Trivers, 1972; Clutton-Brock, 1991; Gross, 2005). Among other benefits, nest-attending parents can increase their fitness by reducing embryonic predation [frogs (Townsend, 1986)], improving egg water balance [skinks (Somma, 1989)], thermoregulating embryos [bumblebees (Heinrich, 1979)], promoting embryonic respiration [fish (Lissaker and kVarnemo, 2006)], reducing pathogen infiltration of eggs [crickets (West and Alexander, 1963)] and provisioning offspring with food [birds (Clutton-Brock, 1991)]. As a result of this wide assortment of benefits, nest-attending behavior is taxonomically widespread (Clutton-Brock, 1991). While birds typically use bi-parental care, female-only parental care is the predominant mode of care in other internally fertilizing vertebrates [e.g. reptiles and mammals (Clutton-Brock, 1991)], including species within major taxa in which external fertilization predominates [i.e. fish and amphibians (Gross and Shine, 1981)], as well as terrestrial arthropods (Zeh and Smith, 1985).

Within the broad context of female-only nest attendance, python egg brooding has recently proven itself as a useful and relevant parental care model. Python egg brooding is simple, yet it affects several critical and quantitatively measurable developmental variables, including embryonic predation (Madsen and Shine, 1999), thermoregulation (Vinegar et al., 1970; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a), water balance (Aubret et al., 2005a; Lourdais et al., 2007; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008) and respiration (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008). While the benefits of python egg brooding have been well established, egg brooding limits nest–clutch respiratory gas exchange, which entails short-term metabolic and long-term phenotypic costs to offspring (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009b). Thus, python egg brooding can be used to examine the physiological trade-offs of parental care.

We have shown in Children's pythons [Antaresia childreni (Gray 1842)] that python egg brooding is dynamic. During egg brooding, females predominately adopt a tightly coiled posture that reduces the exchange of heat, water vapor, O2 and CO2 between the nest and clutch environment that benefits water balance at the cost of embryonic respiration (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008). Hence, females periodically adjust their egg-brooding postures to facilitate nest–clutch gas (O2 and H2O vapor) exchange to benefit respiration at the cost of embryonic water balance (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008).

While postural adjustments are used to balance the various embryonic needs even under static environmental conditions, incubation conditions vary over time (e.g. diel temperature shifts). Thus, it would seem beneficial to the developing offspring if females adjusted the timing and duration of postural adjustments based on environmental inputs. However, while embryonic oxygen consumption increases dramatically over the course of development, female A. childreni do not alter the relative frequency or duration of their postural adjustments, which results in developmental hypoxia that reduces offspring size, speed and strength (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008). Contrarily, we have recently shown that egg-brooding females are capable of assessing the nest–clutch temperature gradient, and they make behavioral adjustments to enhance the thermal micro-environment of their developing offspring. That is, females coil tightly around eggs less often when the nest is warming compared with when it is cooling (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a).

Because of the contrasting results found in female response to variation in environmental temperature and respiratory gas concentrations, we designed an experiment to evaluate female response to another critical variable associated with brooding – water balance. Thus, we designed a multifactorial experiment to assess the relative importance of environmental temperature and hydric condition, two variables critically important to embryonic python development (Shine et al., 1997; Lourdais et al., 2007). We manipulated nest temperature and humidity while monitoring nest–clutch thermal, hydric and respiratory relationships to test the hypothesis that females modify their egg-brooding behavior due to an interaction between environmental thermal and hydric conditions. Our results will specifically provide further understanding of the dynamic nature of python egg brooding and, in general, build upon existing knowledge of the environmental influences on parental behaviors.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study species and reproductive husbandry

For this study, we used a long-term captive colony of A. childreni maintained at Arizona State University (ASU), Tempe, AZ, USA. Antaresia childreni are non-venomous, constricting snakes that inhabit rocky areas in northern Australia (Wilson and Swan, 2003). Husbandry and breeding of the animals followed that described previously (Lourdais et al., 2007). All procedures used in this study were approved by the ASU Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #08-967R).

As described previously (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a), we housed brooding females in substrate-free, Teflon-coated 1.9 l chambers a few days prior to oviposition through the completion of all experimental trials to minimize disturbance, avoid clutch abandonment and ensure the exchange of respiratory gases (i.e. H2O vapor, O2 and CO2) to and from the brooding unit (i.e. female and associated clutch). When not being used in an experimental trial, these brooding chambers were kept in an environmentally controlled room that had a 14 h:10 h L:D photo regime and maintained temperature at the species' preferred developmental temperature (31.5±0.3°C) (Lourdais et al., 2008) to preclude the need for any behavioral thermoregulation by females. Also, we delivered 20–40 ml min−1 of hydrated air [absolute humidity=25.1−26.7 g m−3; relative humidity (RH)=80–85%] to each brooding chamber using methods described previously (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a). At oviposition, we briefly removed each female from her clutch to determine clutch size, clutch mass and female post-oviposition mass.

Experimental procedure

To test our hypothesis, we evaluated real-time egg-brooding behavior, rates of brooding unit oxygen consumption (Embedded Image), carbon dioxide production (Embedded Image) and water loss (MH2O), nest temperature (Tnest) and clutch temperature (Tclutch) of eight A. childreni brooding units [female mass (means ± s.e.m.): 359.5±20.4g; clutch size: 9±1 eggs; absolute clutch mass: 101.6±7.3 g; relative clutch mass (i.e. 100 × clutch mass / maternal mass): 28.4±1.7%] using a flow-through system. We evaluated each brooding unit during consecutive 4-h treatments of four temperature conditions [31.5°C, cooling (1.4°C h−1), 26.0°C, and warming (1.4°C h−1)], and we repeated these trials at three influent humidity conditions assigned in random order [dew points (DP) of 31°C, 25°C and 16°C (13, 23 and 32 g m−3 H2O, respectively)] (Fig. 1). Because field data do not exist for A. childreni nests, we used a thermal regime that represents a contracted diel thermal cycle from nests of water pythons, Liasis fuscus, which are sympatric with A. childreni (Madsen and Shine, 1999). Further, we used humidity information from a database maintained by the Commonwealth of Australia's Bureau of Meteorology to create ecologically relevant nest humidity treatments. Previously, we determined that A. childreni brooding behavior does not have a diel cycle (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008), so the effect of the sequential treatments would not be confounded by time of day. Also, as egg-brooding behavior does not change over incubation (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008), we conducted all trials <1 week post-oviposition. To avoid disturbance, we monitored trials in darkness with an infrared camera and recorded real-time video for later analysis of brooding behavior variables as previously described (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008; Stahlschmidt et al., 2009a).

We conducted experimental trials in a modified 150 l thermoelectric cooler controlled by a datalogger (21X, Campbell Scientific Instruments, Logan, UT, USA) that produced the thermal cycle. We created influent air of known humidity by bubbling building supply air through a heated water column and then sending the air through a condensation chamber held at the desired DP (e.g. 16°C, 25°C or 31°C DP). For the 31°C DP trials, the condensation chamber temperature mimicked that of the trial thermal cycle to maintain a saturated influent air without condensation on the eggs (Fig. 1). We verified the humidity of influent air with a precision hygrometer (RH100, Sable Systems, Las Vegas, NV, USA) positioned immediately upstream of the brooding chamber. We maintained an influent flow rate of 500 ml min−1 with an upstream pressure regulator, an adjustable rotameter and a flow tube (FL-344, Omega Instruments, Stamford, CT, USA) that was calibrated under experimental conditions.

We analyzed a baseline sample of influent air immediately before and after brooding trials and averaged the two to determine the O2 and CO2 concentrations of influent air (the difference between initial and final baseline samples was negligible at 0.0042±0.00081%). Baseline air and air exiting each chamber (effluent air) was passed through a precision hygrometer (RH300, Sable Systems) and dried by anhydrous CaSO4 before flowing through a CO2 analyzer (LI-6252, Li-Cor Biosciences, Lincoln, NE, USA) and an O2 analyzer (FC-1B, Sable Systems) that we calibrated to manufacturer specifications prior to experimental use. During trials, we recorded the O2, CO2 and H2O concentrations of effluent air every minute using a Campbell 23X datalogger. We converted these raw data to Embedded Image, Embedded Image and MH2O using equations 1–7 in Walsberg and Hoffman (Walsberg and Hoffman, 2006). This flow-through respirometry system has a demonstrated accuracy of 0.4% in determining steady-state Embedded Image and Embedded Image (Walsberg and Hoffman, 2005).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Eight Antaresia childreni brooding units were monitored during a continuous sequence of four experimental temperature conditions [constant preferred incubation temperature (horizontal shading), cooling (dotted shading), constant cool temperature (diagonal shading) and warming (vertical shading)], and we repeated these trials under three humidity conditions [31°C dew point (DP) or vapor-saturated, 25°C DP and 16°C DP].

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

The effects of temperature and humidity treatments on the percentage of time female A. childreni spent tightly coiled around their clutches (means ± s.e.m., N=8)

To measure real-time clutch temperature (Tclutch), we positioned a Type-T thermocouple 1–2 cm into the clutch using an access port in the bottom of the brooding chamber, and interfaced it with the 23X datalogger (Campbell Scientific Instruments). We positioned another Type-T thermocouple 1–2 cm inside each brooding chamber's influx port, interfaced it with the Campbell 23X datalogger, and used it to measure nest temperature (Tnest) in real-time. Together, these temperatures allowed us to determine the Tnest–Tclutch gradient (i.e. the difference between mean Tnest and mean Tclutch) at each treatment. Given the dynamic nature of our experimental design, we were able to determine the effect(s) of several thermal and hydric variables on egg-brooding behavior. In addition to Tnest condition (i.e. 31.5°C, cooling, 26.0°C and warming) and nest humidity (31°C, 25°C and 16°C DP), we evaluated the effect(s) of the Tnest–Tclutch gradient (i.e. negative, zero and positive) and the nest–clutch vapor pressure gradient [i.e. low (<0.01 kPa), medium (0.01–1.45 kPa) and high (1.46–2.82 kPa)] on brooding behavior. This multi-factor approach allowed us to determine the relative importance of each variable on brooding behavior.

As described previously, we categorized egg brooding into two behavior types that are strongly associated with nest–clutch thermal, hydric and respiratory dynamics (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008). We defined tight brooding to be when a female was motionless and tightly coiled around her clutch. We considered postural adjustments as individual behavioral events only if they were >30 s removed from another postural adjustment.

Statistical analyses

We verified that our data met the appropriate statistical assumptions of parametric statistics, or we transformed them as necessary. We analyzed all data with SPSS (version 15, SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and determined significance at α<0.05 for all tests. To determine the independent and interactive effects of treatment (i.e. temperature condition, Tnest–Tclutch gradient, nest humidity and the nest–clutch vapor pressure gradient), we used two-factor repeated-measures analysis of variance (RMANOVA) tests. If sphericity was significant, we used Huynh-Feldt epsilon adjustments. We used Bonferroni-corrected paired t-tests for post-hoc analyses. To test relationships among individuals within treatments (e.g. the effect of tight coiling on brooding unit Embedded Image during 31.5°C temperature and 26°C DP conditions of the nest), we used simple linear regression analysis. We present all results as means ± s.e.m., and they refer to comparisons among eight individuals (i.e. N=8).

RESULTS

Coiling behavior was significantly influenced by all temperature and humidity treatments, and there was an effect of the temperature × humidity interaction (Tables 1, 2; Fig. 2). Because animals did not experience a high nest–clutch vapor pressure gradient when nest temperature was 26.0°C, we could not determine the effects of the nest–clutch vapor pressure gradient + temperature condition on coiling behavior. Post-hoc analyses support previously reported findings in that brooding females held at a 25°C DP spent more time coiling tightly when the nest was cooling than when it was warming (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a) (Fig. 2). We obtained similar results for brooding females during vapor-saturated nest conditions (i.e. the 31°C DP treatment, Fig. 2). However, this pattern did not persist under dry conditions, as brooding females at 16°C DP showed a high frequency of tight coiling even when the nest was warming (Fig. 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Repeated-measures analysis of variance results for the independent and interactive effects of temperature and humidity treatments on the percentage of time female A. childreni spent tightly coiled around their clutches (N=8)

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

The effect of humidity on the mean percentage of time female Antaresia childreni spent tightly coiled (% TC) during trials utilizing four sequential temperature conditions. Error bars represent s.e.m., and significant between-temperature differences for the 25°C DP and 31°C DP trials are denoted by boxed * and †, respectively (N=8). Note the significant decrease in % TC during the warming stage for both the 25°C DP and 31°C DP trials but the constant high rate of % TC during the drier 16°C DP trials.

In agreement with a previous study (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008), tight coiling (TC) conserved embryonic water because the percentage of time that a female spent tightly coiled (% TC) was negatively related to brooding unit MH2O during 16°C DP and 25°C DP nest conditions (Table 3) [note: exposed python eggs lose water ~10-fold faster than females alone so the vast majority of brooding unit MH2O is derived from the eggs during postural adjustments (Stahlschmidt et al., 2008)]. As in Stahlschmidt and DeNardo (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a), % TC was significantly and positively related to mean Tnest–Tclutch gradient during warming at 25°C and 31°C DP treatments (Table 4). Low sample size and low among-individual variation in % TC may have contributed to the lack of significance during other warming and cooling treatments as % TC was positively, although non-significantly, related to mean Tnest–Tclutch gradient in all of these instances (Table 4).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Significant relationships between the amount of time female Antaresia childreni (N=8) spent tightly coiled (% TC) and brooding unit oxygen consumption rate. Relationships during other treatments were non-significant possibly due to low sample size and low among-individual variation in % TC (Tables 3 and 4).

Respiratory exchange ratios (i.e. RER, Embedded Image/Embedded Image, 0.82±0.04) were not affected by temperature condition (F3,21=1.5, P=0.25, 1-β=0.33), DP (F1,7=0.011, P=0.92, 1-β=0.051) or the temperature × DP interaction (F2.8,19.5=2.4, P=0.11, 1-β=0.49). Postural adjustments seemed to come with an energetic cost to females as tight coiling behavior was negatively related to brooding unit Embedded Image during several treatments (Fig. 3), particularly when among-individual variance of % TC was high (Tables 3 and 4). The disparity in brooding unit Embedded Image is primarily due to changes in maternal Embedded Image because early-stage A. childreni embryos are not O2 limited and the clutch consumes ~50% less O2 than the brooding female (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2008; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008). Again, low sample size and low among-individual variation in % TC may have contributed to the lack of significance during other treatments as % TC was similarly, although non-significantly, related to brooding unit Embedded Image in all of these instances (all non-significant R2=0.19−0.48; P=0.057−0.28).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

Relationships between the amount of time female A. childreni (N=8) spent tightly coiled (% TC) and brooding unit water loss rates (MH20, mg h−1 g−1) during 16°C dew point (DP) and 25°C DP nest conditions

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 4

Relationships between the amount of time female A. childreni (N=8) spent tightly coiled (% TC) and the mean nest–clutch temperature gradients during warming and cooling nest conditions

DISCUSSION

We experimentally demonstrate that both Tnest and humidity conditions influence python egg-brooding behavior, and that these two effects significantly interact with one another. Also, in agreement with previous studies, the tightly coiled brooding posture reduced nest–clutch exchange of water vapor and heat, particularly during unfavorable thermal and hydric nest conditions (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a; Stahlschmidt et al., 2008). Thus, python egg-brooding postural adjustments are functionally significant to embryonic thermoregulation and water balance because these movements modulate and respond to these two important developmental variables.

Not unexpectedly, many animals significantly invest into the thermoregulation and water balance of their offspring because these aspects are crucial to development and survival (Clutton-Brock, 1991). Although taxonomically and functionally (i.e. behaviorally and physiologically) diverse, parental care adaptations that enhance these variables tend to fall within two simple categories – buffering and provisioning. First, parents can improve the fitness of their offspring by simply providing resistance to embryonic water or heat loss through building nests and synthesizing eggshells or egg coats (Clutton-Brock, 1991). Second, parents can enhance embryonic thermoregulation and water balance by providing heat or water to their embryos. Examples include viviparity (i.e. live-bearing, which allows parents to directly regulate embryonic temperature and hydration), endothermic brooding and parental feeding (Clutton-Brock, 1991). To optimize offspring fitness, pythons use either buffering alone [e.g. A. childreni (Stahlschmidt and DeNardo, 2009a); this study] or both buffering and provisioning [e.g. Python molurus (Hutchison et al., 1966; Vinegar et al., 1970)].

Given embryos' sensitivity to incubation conditions (Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Deeming, 2004), parental behaviors are often modified in response to environmental thermal or hydric dynamics. For example, ambient temperature (Ta) influences the percentage of time female little stints (Calidris minuta) spend brooding and foraging (i.e. females brood more when Ta is low) (Tulp et al., 2009). To reduce egg desiccation, female prairie skinks (Eumeces septentrionalis) increase their egg-brooding behaviors when the nest substrate is relatively dry (Somma and Fawcett, 1989). In addition to shifts in brooding behaviors, several reptile species demonstrate adaptive nest site selection related to thermal or hydric conditions [e.g. lizards (Shine and Harlow, 1996), turtles (Belinsky et al., 2004), snakes (Brown and Shine, 2004)]. The role of hygrosensation in adaptive nest site selection is taxonomically widespread. For example, Montell demonstrated that fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) enhance the fitness of their offspring by preferentially ovipositing on moist substrate (Montell, 2008). Our results indicate that python egg-brooding behavior is adaptively plastic in response to both nest thermal and hydric dynamics.

While highly beneficial to the offspring, parental care generally entails substantial energy-related costs to the parent(s) (Clutton-Brock, 1991). In pythons, egg brooding is generally accompanied by lost foraging time and anorexia (Madsen and Shine, 1999; Aubret et al., 2005b). Accordingly, egg brooding obligates significant epaxial muscle atrophy and reduces contraction strength in female A. childreni under laboratory conditions (O. Lourdais and D.F.D., unpublished). However, other research suggests that brooding-related maternal costs are minimal (Aubret et al., 2005b). Interestingly, some water pythons nest in root boles where they brood their eggs for the duration of incubation (mean: 58 days) while other females nest in more thermally stable varanid burrows and only brood their eggs for the first week of incubation (Madsen and Shine, 1999). Females that use root boles and thus brood throughout incubation have reduced reproductive body condition and survival rate relative to those females that nest in burrows (Madsen and Shine, 1999).

Similar to large-scale decisions (i.e. to brood or not to brood the clutch), finer-scale egg-brooding decisions (i.e. the frequency of postural adjustments) also entail energetic costs. In fact, of the brooding units in this study, the one with the most behaviorally active female (i.e. lowest % TC) consumed >3-fold more O2 g−1 body mass than the brooding unit that had the least active female when the nest was warming and vapor-saturated. The energetic cost of postural adjustments is probably relatively low compared with the energy demands of other aspects of parental investment (e.g. yolk deposition and lost foraging time) but warrants further investigation.

To conclude, our results combined with those of previous studies (e.g. Madsen and Shine, 1999) demonstrate that female pythons make both large-scale and fine-scale parental decisions. In addition to deciding where to brood and for how long, female pythons can also alter their brooding behavior. Thus, while providing a less complex form of parental care compared with that of mammals and birds, female pythons assess and respond to specific nest conditions to optimize the developmental environment experienced by their offspring. Although python egg brooding is emerging as a simple yet valuable parental care model, many critical questions regarding this system remain unanswered. Future research should focus on the proximate sensory mechanisms of egg-brooding decision-making such as neuronal pathways [e.g. transient receptor potential channels (Romanovsky, 2007; Montell, 2008)] and hormonal regulation (e.g. estradiol, progesterone, thyroxin and prolactin). Additionally, while challenging, field studies that integrate the various levels of parental decision-making (e.g. nest site selection, brooding duration and postural adjustment frequency) would provide critical insight.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Glenn Walsberg and Jake Brashears for technical support and the National Science Foundation (IOS-0543979 to D.F.D. and a Graduate Research Fellowship to Z.R.S.) for financial support. We are grateful to Ernest Nigro for attentive animal husbandry and those who helped with the preparation of the manuscript, including Kevin McGraw and two anonymous reviewers.

  • © 2010.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Aubret, F.,
    2. Bonnet, X.,
    3. Shine, R. and
    4. Maumelat, S.
    (2005a). Why do female ball pythons (Python regius) coil so tightly around their eggs? Evol. Ecol. Res. 7, 743-758.
    OpenUrl
  2. ↵
    1. Aubret, F.,
    2. Bonnet, X.,
    3. Shine, R. and
    4. Maumelat, S.
    (2005b). Energy expenditure for parental care may be trivial for brooding pythons, Python regius. Anim. Beh. 69, 1043-1053.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Belinsky, A.,
    2. Ackerman, R. A.,
    3. Dmi'el, R. and
    4. Ar, A.
    (2004). Water in reptilian eggs and hatchlings. In Reptilian Incubation: Environment, Evolution, and Behaviour (ed. Deeming, D. C.), pp. 125-141. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.
  4. ↵
    1. Brown, G. P. and
    2. Shine, R.
    (2004). Maternal nest-site choice and offspring fitness in a tropical snake (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae). Ecology 85, 1627-1634.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    1. Clutton-Brock, T. H.
    (1991). The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  6. ↵
    1. Deeming, D. C.
    (2004). Reptilian Incubation: Environment, Evolution, and Behaviour. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.
  7. ↵
    1. Deeming, D. C. and
    2. Ferguson, M. W. J.
    (1991). Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic Development in Birds and Reptiles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  8. ↵
    1. Gross, M. R.
    (2005). The evolution of parental care. Q. Rev. Biol. 80, 37-45.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Gross, M. R. and
    2. Shine, R.
    (1981). Parental care and mode of fertilization in ectothermic vertebrates. Evolution 35, 775-793.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  10. ↵
    1. Heinrich, B.
    (1979). Bumblebee Economics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  11. ↵
    1. Hutchison, V. H.,
    2. Dowling, H. G. and
    3. Vinegar, A.
    (1966). Thermoregulation in a brooding female Indian python, Python molurus bivittatus. Science 151, 694-696.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Lissaker, M. and
    2. ·Kvarnemo, C.
    (2006). Ventilation or nest defense – parental care trade-offs in a fish with male care. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 60, 864-873.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Lourdais, O.,
    2. Hoffman, T. C. M. and
    3. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2007). Maternal brooding in the children's python (Antaresia childreni) promotes egg water balance. J. Comp. Physiol. B 177, 569-577.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Lourdais, O.,
    2. Heulin, B. and
    3. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2008). Thermoregulation during gravidity in the Children's python (Antaresia childreni): a test of the pre-adaptation hypothesis for maternal thermophily in snakes. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 93, 499-508.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Madsen, T. and
    2. Shine, R.
    (1999). Life history consequences of nest-site variation in tropical pythons. Ecology 80, 989-997.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    1. Montell, C.
    (2008). TRP channels: it's not the heat, it's the humidity. Curr. Biol. 18, R123-R126.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Romanovsky, A. A.
    (2007). Thermoregulation: some concepts have changed. Functional architecture of the thermoregulatory system. Am. J. Physiol - Regl. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 292, R37-R46.
    OpenUrl
  18. ↵
    1. Shine, R. and
    2. Harlow, P. S.
    (1996). Maternal manipulation of offspring phenotypes via nest-site selection in an oviparous lizard. Ecology 77, 1808-1817.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Shine, R.,
    2. Madsen, T. R. L.,
    3. Elphick, M. J. and
    4. Harlow, P. S.
    (1997). The influence of nest temperatures and maternal brooding on hatchling phenotypes in water pythons. Ecology 78, 1713-1721.
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
  20. ↵
    1. Somma, L. A. and
    2. Fawcett, J. D.
    (1989). Brooding behavior of the prairie skink and its relationship to the hydric environment of the nest. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 95, 245-256.
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    1. Stahlschmidt, Z. R. and
    2. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2008). Alternating egg brooding behaviors create and modulate a hypoxic developmental micro-environment in Children's pythons (Antaresia childreni). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 1535-1540.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Stahlschmidt, Z. R. and
    2. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2009a). Effect of nest temperature on egg-brooding dynamics in Children's pythons. Physiol. Beh. 98, 302-306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Stahlschmidt, Z. R. and
    2. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2009b). Obligate costs of parental care to offspring: Egg brooding induced hypoxia creates smaller, slower, and weaker python offspring. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 98, 414-421.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Stahlschmidt, Z. R.,
    2. Hoffman, T. C. M. and
    3. DeNardo, D. F.
    (2008). Postural shifts during egg-brooding and their impact on egg water balance in Children's pythons (Antaresia childreni). Ethology 114, 1113-1121.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Townsend, D. S.
    (1986). The costs of male parental care and its evolution in a Neotropical frog. Beh. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19, 187-195.
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    1. Trivers, R. L.
    (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man (ed. Campbell, B.), pp. 136-179. Chicago: Aldine.
  27. ↵
    1. Tulp, I.,
    2. Schekkerman, H.,
    3. Bruinzeel, L. W.,
    4. Jukema, J.,
    5. Visser, G. H. and
    6. Piersma, T.
    (2009). Energetic demands during incubation and chick rearing in a uniparental and biparental shorebird breeding in the high Arctic. Auk. 126, 155-164.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Vinegar, A.,
    2. Hutchison, V. H. and
    3. Dowling, H. G.
    (1970). Metabolism, energetics, and thermoregulation during brooding of snakes of genus Python (Reptilia, Boidae). Zoologica. 55, 19-48.
    OpenUrl
  29. ↵
    1. Walsberg, G. E. and
    2. Hoffman, T. C. M.
    (2005). Direct calorimetry reveals large errors in respirometric estimates of energy expenditure. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 1035-1043.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Walsberg, G. E. and
    2. Hoffman, T. C. M.
    (2006). Using direct calorimetry to test the accuracy of indirect calorimetry in an ectotherm. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 79, 830-835.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. West, K. J. and
    2. Alexander, R. D.
    (1963). Sub-social behavior in a burrowing cricket Anurogryllus muticus (De Geer). Orthoptera: Gryllidae. Ohio J. Sci. 63, 19-24.
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    1. Williams, G. G.
    (1966). Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack's principle. Am. Nat. 100, 687-690.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  33. ↵
    1. Wilson, S. and
    2. Swan, G.
    (2003). Reptiles of Australia. Princeton: Princeton Field Guides.
  34. ↵
    1. Zeh, D. W. and
    2. Smith, R. L.
    (1985). Paternal investment by terrestrial arthropods. Am. Zool. 25, 785-805.
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Parental behavior in pythons is responsive to both the hydric and thermal dynamics of the nest
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
Parental behavior in pythons is responsive to both the hydric and thermal dynamics of the nest
Zachary Stahlschmidt, Dale F. DeNardo
Journal of Experimental Biology 2010 213: 1691-1696; doi: 10.1242/jeb.041095
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
Parental behavior in pythons is responsive to both the hydric and thermal dynamics of the nest
Zachary Stahlschmidt, Dale F. DeNardo
Journal of Experimental Biology 2010 213: 1691-1696; doi: 10.1242/jeb.041095

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • INTRODUCTION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Unexpected lack of specialisation in the flow properties of spitting cobra venom
  • Responses of Manduca sexta larvae to heat waves
  • Heat hardening in a pair of Anolis lizards: constraints, dynamics and ecological consequences
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLES

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Predicting the Future: Species Survival in a Changing World

Read our new special issue exploring the significant role of experimental biology in assessing and predicting the susceptibility or resilience of species to future, human-induced environmental change.


Big Biology Podcast - Hollie Putnam and coral bleaching

Catch the next JEB-sponsored episode of the Big Biology Podcast where Art and Marty talk to Hollie Putnam about the causes of coral bleaching and the basic biology of corals in the hope of selectively breeding corals that can better tolerate future ocean conditions.

Read Hollie's Review on the subject, which is featured in our current special issue. 


Stark trade-offs and elegant solutions in arthropod visual systems

Many elegant eye specializations that evolved in response to visual challenges continue to be discovered. A new Review by Meece et al. summarises exciting solutions evolved by insects and other arthropods in response to specific visual challenges.


Head bobbing gives pigeons a sense of perspective

Pigeons might look goofy with their head-bobbing walk, but it turns out that the ungainly head manoeuvre allows the birds to judge distance.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992