Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Novel sensory modalities for navigation and other behaviours
Seeing in the dark: vision and visual behaviour in nocturnal bees and wasps
Eric J. Warrant
Journal of Experimental Biology 2008 211: 1737-1746; doi: 10.1242/jeb.015396
Eric J. Warrant
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Tables

Figures

  • Fig. 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 1.

    Compound eye designs. (A) A focal apposition compound eye. Light reaches the photoreceptors exclusively from the small corneal lens located directly above. This eye design is typical of day-active insects. (B) A refracting superposition compound eye. A large number of corneal facets and bullet-shaped crystalline cones collect and focus light – across the clear zone of the eye (cz) – towards single photoreceptors in the retina. Several hundred, or even thousand, facets service a single photoreceptor. Not surprisingly, many nocturnal and deep-sea animals have refracting superposition eyes, and benefit from the significant improvement in sensitivity. Diagrams courtesy of Dan-Eric Nilsson. Adapted from Warrant (Warrant, 2004).

  • Fig. 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 2.

    Nocturnal bees and wasps. (A) The Central American sweat bee Megalopta genalis (Halictidae), whose sensitive apposition eyes allow them to forage at night by visually learning landmarks along the foraging route and around the nest entrance. Reproduced with the kind permission of the photographer, Dr Michael Pfaff. (B,C) The pale-yellow coloured Central American paper wasp Apoica pallens (Vespidae), which congregates on the outside of the nest (B) to create a distinctive pale object that may be visible to returning foragers at night. Photographs in B and C were reproduced with the kind permission of the photographer, Gillian Little, and Daniel Marlos from `What's That Bug?', a website devoted to popular entomology (www.whatsthatbug.com).

  • Fig. 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 3.

    The daily timing of foraging flights in the nocturnal bee Megalopta genalis. Megalopta is active during two short time intervals each day, once in the morning (A) and once in the evening (B), when light levels at the nest (blue symbols) can be lower than 10–4 cd m–2. The number of bees leaving the nest (green bars) and returning to the nest (red bars) in 5 min intervals is shown relative to sunrise or sunset (0:00 h). Data were collected over several nights from several nests during two successive years on Barro Colorado Island (Panama). (A) The timing of foraging trips in the morning. The grey area indicates the time before astronomical twilight. (B) The timing of foraging trips in the evening. The grey area indicates the time after astronomical twilight. During the time interval between the onset of astronomical twilight in the evening and its offset in the morning, no light from the sun is present in the night sky. Modified with kind permission from Kelber et al. (Kelber et al., 2006).

  • Fig. 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 4.

    Nocturnal landmark orientation in the nocturnal halictid bee Megalopta genalis. (A) A typical nocturnal orientation flight, as seen from below. The bee leaves her nest, and quickly returns to face the nest entrance. Flying in short arcs, she investigates the nest entrance and a neighbouring landmark to learn their spatial arrangement before departing on her foraging trip. Each `ball-and-stick' represents the position of the head (ball) and body (stick) at 40 ms intervals. (B,C) Landmark learning. Bees leaving for a foraging trip learn the position of their nest relative to others (B), or learn the presence of a white square card attached to their nest (C). Upon return, bees enter the nest marked by the landmarks they have previously learned, not their actual nests (which are marked by stars). The rear side of the square card was attached to a Perspex cylinder that slipped neatly over the end of the nest stick to hold the card in place over the nest entrance. Times and light intensities at departure and return are also shown. Adapted from Warrant et al. (Warrant et al., 2004).

  • Fig. 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 5.

    The flight paths of four individual nocturnal bees (Megalopta genalis) returning to the nest at dimmer (A,D) and brighter (B,C) light levels during dawn (A,B) and dusk (C,D). In each plot, the three-dimensional flight path (black) is shown together with two-dimensional projections (grey shadows on the right, left and bottom walls) of the flight path onto the imaginary walls of a cubical space centred on the nest entrance (which is shown as a cylinder on the right wall). Luminance in the early dawn (A) was 1.1×10–4 cd m–2 and the landing lasted 11.4 s; late dawn (B), 1.9×10–3 cd m–2 and the landing lasted 4.7 s; early dusk (C), 3.9×10–3 cd m–2 and the landing lasted 1.8 s; and late dusk (D), 3.9×10–4 cd m–2 and the landing lasted 16.2 s. Note that during both the dawn and the dusk, landing flights were more circuitous and took longer in dimmer light. Each grid square is 10 cm×10 cm. Adapted with kind permission from Theobald et al. (Theobald et al., 2007).

  • Fig. 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 6.

    Adaptations for nocturnal vision in the photoreceptors of the nocturnal sweat bee Megalopta genalis, as compared to photoreceptors in the closely related diurnal sweat bee Lasioglossum leucozonium. (A,B) Responses to single photons (or `photon bumps': arrowheads) recorded from photoreceptors in Megalopta (A) and Lasioglossum (B). Note that the bump amplitude is larger, and the bump time course much slower, in Megalopta than in Lasioglossum. (C–F) Average contrast gain as a function of temporal frequency in Megalopta (blue curves, N=8 cells) and Lasioglossum (red curves, N=8 cells) at different adapting intensities, indicated as `effective photons' per second in each panel for each species [for each species, each stimulus intensity was calibrated in terms of `effective photons'; that is, the number of photon bumps per second the light source elicited, thereby eliminating the effects of differences in the light-gathering capacity of the optics between the two species, which is about 27 times (Lillywhite and Laughlin, 1979)]. In light-adapted conditions (C,D), the two species reach the same maximum contrast gain per unit bandwidth although Lasioglossum has a broader bandwidth and a higher corner frequency (the frequency at which the gain has fallen off to 50% of its maximum). In dark-adapted conditions (E,F), Megalopta has a much higher contrast gain per unit bandwidth. All panels adapted with kind permission from Frederiksen et al. (Frederiksen et al., 2008).

  • Fig. 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 7.

    The average rates of information transmission (in bits s–1) in the photoreceptors of the nocturnal and diurnal sweat bees Megalopta genalis (blue curves, N=8 cells) and Lasioglossum leucozonium (red curves, N=8 cells). (A) When the photoreceptors alone are considered (via a light source calibration in `effective photons' absorbed by the photoreceptor per second), it is evident that at all intensities Lasioglossum has a higher information rate than Megalopta. (B) When light sources are instead calibrated to external ambient intensities (a normalised intensity of 100 corresponds to the light intensity on an overcast day, or around 180 cd m–2), Megalopta has a higher information rate in dim light. This, however, is due to its 27 times more sensitive optics and is not due to an intrinsic adaptation present within the photoreceptors. Error bars show ±s.d. Both panels adapted with kind permission from Frederiksen et al. (Frederiksen et al., 2008).

  • Fig. 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 8.

    Spatial summation in nocturnal bees. (A) Comparison of the first-order interneurons – L-fibre types L2, L3 and L4 – of the Megalopta genalis female (left) and the worker honeybee Apis mellifera (right). Compared with the worker honeybee, the horizontal branches of L-fibres in the nocturnal halictid bee connect to a much larger number of lamina cartridges, suggesting a possible role in spatial summation. L, lamina; M, medulla. Reconstructions from Golgi-stained frontal sections. Adapted from Greiner et al. (Greiner et al., 2004b) and Ribi (Ribi, 1975). (B,C) Spatial and temporal summation modelled at different light intensities in Megalopta genalis (B) and Apis mellifera (C) for an image velocity (V) of 240° s–1 [measured from Megalopta genalis during a nocturnal foraging flight (Warrant et al., 2004)]. Light intensities are given for 540 nm, the peak in the bee's spectral sensitivity. Equivalent natural intensities are also shown. The finest spatial detail visible to flying bees (as measured by the maximum detectable spatial frequency, νmax) is plotted as a function of light intensity. When bees sum photons optimally in space and time (continuous lines), vision is extended to much lower light intensities (non-zeroν max) compared with when summation is absent (broken lines). Note that nocturnal bees can see in dimmer light than honeybees. Grey areas denote the light intensity window within which each species is normally active (although honeybees are also active at intensities higher than those presented on the graph). Adapted from Theobald et al. (Theobald et al., 2006).

Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Seeing in the dark: vision and visual behaviour in nocturnal bees and wasps
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Novel sensory modalities for navigation and other behaviours
Seeing in the dark: vision and visual behaviour in nocturnal bees and wasps
Eric J. Warrant
Journal of Experimental Biology 2008 211: 1737-1746; doi: 10.1242/jeb.015396
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Novel sensory modalities for navigation and other behaviours
Seeing in the dark: vision and visual behaviour in nocturnal bees and wasps
Eric J. Warrant
Journal of Experimental Biology 2008 211: 1737-1746; doi: 10.1242/jeb.015396

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • Introduction
    • Nocturnality in bees and wasps
    • Nocturnal visual behaviour in bees and wasps
    • Visual adaptations for reliable nocturnal vision
    • Conclusions
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • The sensory ecology of ocean navigation
  • Integrative biology of an embryonic respiratory behaviour in pond snails: the `embryo stir-bar hypothesis'
Show more Novel sensory modalities for navigation and other behaviours

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


The Big Biology podcast

JEB author Christine Cooper talks to Big Biology about her research. In this fascinating JEB sponsored podcast she tells us how tough zebra finches adjust their physiology to cope with extreme climate events. 


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992