Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Commentary
Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression
Cliff H. Summers, Svante Winberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2006 209: 4581-4589; doi: 10.1242/jeb.02565
Cliff H. Summers
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Svante Winberg
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

SUMMARY

Socially aggressive interaction is stressful. What is more, social aggression is stressful for both dominant and subordinate animals. Much of the neurocircuitry for stress and aggression overlap. The pattern of neurochemical and hormonal events stimulated by social interaction make it clear that subtle differences in this pattern of response distinguish social rank. The neurotransmitter serotonin (5-HT) responds rapidly to stress, and also appears to play the most important role for inhibitory regulation of aggressive interactions. In addition, the adrenocortical/interrenal steroid hormones corticosterone and cortisol are responsive to stress and influence aggression. However, while 5-HT and glucocorticoids can both be inhibitory to aggression, the relationship between 5-HT and glucocorticoids is not straightforward, and much of the distinctions in function depend upon timing. Neither is inhibitory during the early stressful phase of aggression. This transmitter-hormone combination follows and influences a four-stage functional pattern of effect: (1) predisposed (positively or negatively) toward aggression, (2) motivated toward behavior, (3) responsive to stress (including aggression) and passively allowing aggression, and finally (4) chronically applied 5-HT and glucocorticoids inhibit aggression.

  • antagonism
  • attack
  • corticosterone
  • cortisol
  • dominant
  • dopamine (DA)
  • fight
  • hostility
  • serotonin (5-HT)
  • social stress
  • stages
  • subordinate
  • timeline

Introduction

Aggressive behavior is manifest in numerous contexts, each of which imparts social or ecological meaning to the kind of antagonism that develops. Social and ecological significance are imparted even prior to aggressive interactions by contextual cues, including the status of the individuals involved. Social standing is influenced in part by previous interactions (Summers et al., 2005a) and the capacity to react to stressful conditions with adaptive neuroendocrine responses (Korzan et al., 2006b). The rate and probability of behavioral responses follows a linear pattern of behavioral, transmitter, and hormonal interaction that result in: (1) predisposition (positively or negatively) toward aggression (Ferrari et al., 2005), (2) motivation for behavior, which includes particular adaptations for coping with social stress (Koolhaas et al., 1999), (3) neuroendocrine responsiveness to the stress of aggression and, for some, (4) chronic social stress that inhibits aggression (Fig. 1).

While the stages of aggressive interaction necessarily follow parameters defined by stress responsiveness and motivation, there is also behavioral and psychological specificity for offensive or defensive aggression, and antagonisms arising from intruders, tormenters, territorial competition, maternal protection or rage. The neurocircuitry and the neurotransmitters and/or modulators involved are almost certainly fine-tuned for specific circumstances. In mammals, the underlying basic circuitry for aggressive behavior, however, appears to be focused on the anterior hypothalamus (Gregg and Siegel, 2001), sometimes including a region described as the attack area (Hrabovszky et al., 2005; Kruk et al., 1979), modified by a number of hypothalamic and extrahypothalamic afferents (Delville et al., 2000; Ferris et al., 1997). While the neurocircuitry for aggression appears to be evolutionarily conserved, the primacy of the anterior hypothalamus has yet to be confirmed in other vertebrates like fish and reptiles. Although there are numerous excitatory inputs to the system, which promote aggressive interaction, the primary regulatory control of aggression is inhibitory (Nelson and Chiavegatto, 2001). The accuracy of this proposition is easily verified by a simple observation: aggression is not unrelenting. Even among territorial animals that must constantly protect resources, most of the time aggressive tendencies are held in check. Revealing aggressive potential is the consequence of numerous provocations in the normal life history of most animals and humans.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

The development of an aggressive social interaction over time progresses through distinct stages of neuroendocrine and behavioral state. In the first phase, prior to interaction baseline levels of plasma glucocorticoids (B=corticosterone in lizards, rats, mice, frogs, birds; F=cortisol in fish, humans, sheep, hamsters) and serotonergic and perhaps dopaminergic activity in brain regions associated with aggression and motivation influence the animal's predisposition (positively or negatively) toward aggression. The prefight neuroendocrine profile is distinctly different in individuals that will become dominant (dom), and those that will become subordinate (sub). At some point, when combatants meet and social engagement begins, early social signals and rapid changes in neuroendocrine profile of putatively dominant individuals generate motivation toward behavior. No aggression has occurred yet, but increased activity of glucocorticoids, serotonin (5-HT) and dopamine (DA) enhance the likelihood of heightened behavioral interaction, culminating in aggression. After aggression begins, both animals are fully responsive to stress, and expressing social signals. Aggression is very stressful, and for that reason, serotonergic and glucocorticoid activities rise dramatically and rapidly in animals that become dominant and those that become subordinate. Surprisingly neither elevated plasma glucocorticoid levels nor serotonergic activity inhibit aggression at this time. Glucocorticoids appear to be necessary for the full expression of early aggression, and elevated B or F plus increased serotonergic activity appear to passively allow aggression. Finally, after social rank is established, chronically elevated or applied 5-HT and glucocorticoids inhibit aggression. Chronic neuroendocrine state likely influences subsequent interactions.

Therefore, in this review, we will consider neural systems that stimulate aggression, but we will focus on inhibitory regulation. That inhibitory regulation emanates primarily from one neurotransmitter system, serotonin (5-HT) (Nelson and Chiavegatto, 2001), but it is far from simple. Its complex nature is derived from neuroendocrine relationships between stress and aggression. The timeline on which these two interacting systems are co-expressed take place before, during and after socially aggressive interaction (Fig. 1). The timeline is suggested as a model of aggressive interaction in all vertebrates, but we especially draw from examples of evolutionarily conserved behavior and neuroendocrine adaptation in fish and lizards (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Summers et al., 2005c). We describe neurotransmitter and hormonal responses during four stages, where they: (1) influence predisposition (positively or negatively) toward aggression, (2) motivate behavior (3) are responsive to stress (including aggression) but passively allow aggression and finally (4) may chronically inhibit aggression.

Aggression neurocircuitry

Experimental inhibitory effects of 5-HT on aggressive behavior are most often the result of systemic application of serotonergic drugs, like selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitors (SSRI; usually a stage 4 type effect). However, a similar effect has been elicited in mammals by applying serotonergic drugs directly to the anterior hypothalamus (Vergnes et al., 1988; Ferris et al., 1999), inhibiting two types of excitatory neurons that stimulate agonistic behavior: AVP interneurons, and excitatory glutamatergic efferents (Hrabovszky et al., 2005) to the periaqueductal gray (PAG)/midbrain central gray (Roeling et al., 1994; Yao et al., 2001). The vasopressinergic neurons of the anterior hypothalamus show increased fos activity during aggression (Delville et al., 2000). This region of the brain can also be directly stimulated (as might happen in stage 3) electrically, hormonally (corticosterone), neurochemically (vasopressin=AVP; glutamate=Glu), and pharmacologically (V1A receptor agonist; Kainate Glu receptor;γ -aminobutyric acid type A receptor=GABAA antagonist) to elicit aggression (Kruk et al., 2004; Kruk et al., 1979; Ferris and Delville, 1994; Ferris et al., 1997; Brody et al., 1969; Gregg and Siegel, 2001; Siegel et al., 1999). The capacity of the anterior hypothalamus to stimulate aggression is so reliable that some investigators refer to it as the `attack area' (Halasz et al., 2002). The anterior hypothalamus appears to provide the central point for integration of forebrain afferents that regulate aggression, and also provides a coordinated excitatory efferent output to the PAG (Roeling et al., 1994; Yao et al., 2001), on the way to motor efferents.

In addition to the internal vasopressinergic stimulation of glutamatergic aggression effectors, there are numerous other excitatory and inhibitory inputs to the anterior hypothalamus from limbic and other forebrain regions. Positive regulation of aggression has been noted for the medial amygdala, lateral septum and mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (Gregg and Siegel, 2001; David et al., 2004; Delville et al., 2000; Halasz et al., 2002). Inhibitory regulation of aggression (modifiable in stage 1) comes most directly from the raphé, which provides serotonergic innervation for most of the forebrain. Some serotonergic neurons of the hamster raphé show increased fos activity during aggression (Delville et al., 2000). In lizards and fish, aggressive interaction powerfully influences the raphé. In the lizard Anolis carolinensis, while serotonergic activity is rapidly increasing in limbic regions during aggressive interaction, it is concomitantly decreased in the raphé (Summers et al., 2003a). Similarly, in sex-changing fish, opposite levels of serotonergic activity are observed in the raphé and limbic terminal regions of saddleback wrasses during the aggressive phase in newly established males, which reverses when the courtship phase ensues (Larson et al., 2003).

Cross linkage of aggression and stress circuitry

There are several lines of evidence that aggression and stress are linked, and that part of the reason that they are reciprocally interrelated is that both mobilize activity in specific brain regions, presumably linked by different but cooperative circuitries. This cross linkage is in play during all four stages of aggressive interaction, modifying predisposition, motivation, and active aggression. This interface across circuitries is apparently developed during fetal through pubertal maturation, as stress-related transmitters such as 5-HT are less effective inhibiting aggression during puberty than in adults (Taravosh-Lahn et al., 2006). When stressful conditions are present during development, effects can be measured in the neural circuitries that regulate both aggression and stress, and in some brain regions those circuitries coincide. Repeated or chronic stress applied during puberty accelerates the progression of development of aggressive behaviors in male hamsters (Wommack et al., 2003; Delville et al., 2003), accompanied by neuroendocrine changes in brain regions like medial amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) that are active in both stress and aggression (Wommack et al., 2004). Stressful conditions such as frustration (absence of reward) stimulates a significant increase in synaptic activity in the neural circuitry controlling aggression (David et al., 2004). Producing stress by removing reward can provoke intense aggression (Gallup, 1965; Azrin et al., 1966; Cherek and Pickens, 1970).

Serotonergic inhibition of aggression

The role of 5-HT and serotonergic circuits in regulating aggressive interaction has been well documented, with a plethora of studies demonstrating that acute or chronic application of 5-HT or serotonergic drugs inhibit aggression in a wide range of vertebrates, including: rainbow trout, coral reef fish, electric fish, the lizard Anolis carolinensis, pigeons, sparrows, hamsters, mice, rats, dogs and humans (see Summers et al., 2005b). In addition, a microdialysis study in rats has demonstrated decreased 5-HT release in prefrontal cortex, but not nucleus accumbens, during aggression (van Erp and Miczek, 2000). However, a variety of studies have also demonstrated increased serotonergic activity in the brain during aggressive interaction (stage 3) (see Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Summers et al., 2005b), often measured in regions of the brain that are known to be involved in regulating aggression, and also increasing at a time when inhibition of aggression would be maladaptive. Additionally, a number of recent studies, looking at specific local changes, or specific receptor activity, cast doubt on inhibitory regulatory effect of 5-HT on aggression (de Boer and Koolhaas, 2005; de Almeida et al., 2005). Even antidepressant treatments, including selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitors, have been demonstrated to increase aggression in rats when administered chronically (Mitchell, 2005; Mitchell and Redfern, 1997; Mitchell and Redfern, 1992). Recent work on the 5-HT3 receptor in hamsters demonstrates that action at this receptor can have both stimulatory and inhibitory effects on aggression (Ricci et al., 2005; Ricci et al., 2004).

In A. carolinensis, inhibitory effects (similar to stages 1 or 4) of serotonergic action have been demonstrated with SSRIs including fluoxetine and sertraline (Deckel, 1996; Larson and Summers, 2001), and 5-HT1, 5-HT2 and 5-HT3 receptor agonists (Deckel and Fuqua, 1998). However, like other vertebrates, serotonergic activity rises rapidly during aggressive interaction in this lizard, and stays elevated for the duration of aggressive interaction (Summers et al., 2003a). The change occurs within 90 s, and has been demonstrated to occur within 30 s in the lizard Sceloporus jarrovi, just when the lizards are initiating combat (stage 3) and being most aggressive (Matter et al., 1998; Summers et al., 2005c). It is also the period when the social sign stimulus (eyespots) is having an effect on social rank formation (Summers, 2002; Summers et al., 2005a). Specific aspects of the stereotypical aggressive displays of this species (Baxter, 2003) increase serotonergic turnover, and activity at 5-HT1B (Baxter, 2001) and 5-HT2C receptors (Baxter et al., 2001b). Although 5-HT does not appear to actively constrain aggression during combat, as serotonergic activity is highest at the peak of aggressive interaction, 5-HT does appear to have an inhibitory effect prior to aggressive behavior commencing (stage 1), based on elevated serotonergic activity measured prior to social interaction in males likely to become subordinate (Summers et al., 2005b; Korzan et al., 2006b). The effect of 5-HT on aggression is found specifically, and only, in regions of aggression neurocircuitry (Baxter, 2001; Baxter et al., 2001b; Summers et al., 2005b). Within that circuitry 5-HT is negatively correlated with aggression in lateral septum, nucleus accumbens, striatum, medial amygdala, anterior hypothalamus, raphé and locus ceruleus. Less aggressive male A. carolinensis have significantly more serotonergic activity in those regions, suggesting that 5-HT actively inhibits the aggressive posture of those males.

As in lizards, dyadic fights for social dominance in rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss initially results in elevated plasma cortisol and brain serotonergic activity in both winners and losers (stage 3), even though winners show no sign of behavioral inhibition (Øverli et al., 1999). Thus, it seems as if only long-term elevation of brain serotonergic activity results in a suppression of aggressive behavior (stages 1 or 4). This suggestion is also supported by the results from a series of studies on the effects of elevated dietary intake of the amino acid L-tryptophan (Trp) on aggressive behavior in rainbow trout. The essential amino acid Trp is the precursor of 5-HT, and in rainbow trout (Aldegunde et al., 2000; Aldegunde et al., 1998; Johnston et al., 1990) as well as in mammals (Moir and Eccleston, 1968; Friedman et al., 1972), the rate of 5-HT synthesis appears to be limited by Trp availability. In juvenile rainbow trout elevated dietary intake of Trp results in suppression of aggressive behavior (as in stage 1), but only after feeding the fish Trp-supplemented feed for 7 days (Winberg et al., 2001). Feeding the fish Trp-supplemented feed for 3 days had no effect on aggressive behavior even though the effects of elevated dietary Trp on brain 5-HT synthesis are rapid (Winberg et al., 2001; Lepage et al., 2003). Moreover, following seven days of supplementary Trp the effects on aggressive behavior were pronounced whereas the effects on brain serotonergic activity were very modest (Lepage et al., 2003; Winberg et al., 2001; Lepage et al., 2005a), suggesting that mechanisms other than a direct effect of Trp on the rate of 5-HT synthesis and release are involved. The inhibitory effects of 5-HT on aggressive behavior have been suggested to be counteracted by an early activation of brain norepinephrine (NE) and dopamine (DA) systems (stages 2 or 3) during agonistic interaction (Höglund et al., 2001; Winberg and Nilsson, 1992; Winberg et al., 1991). However, Trp supplemented feed had no effect on brain DA or NE, and the time course of the effects of Trp on aggressive behavior does not seem to be related to Trp induced effects on these catecholaminergic systems (Lepage et al., 2003). In addition to its relatively modest effects on brain 5-HT, supplementary dietary Trp resulted in elevated plasma levels of melatonin (Lepage et al., 2005b). In fact, this effect was much more pronounced than the effect on 5-HT, and trout receiving Trp supplemented feed displayed drastically elevated daytime plasma melatonin. Although melatonin appears to contribute to aggressive behavior in hamsters (Demas et al., 2004; Jasnow et al., 2002), Trp-stimulated plasma melatonin did not appear to mediate the effects of Trp on aggressive behavior in trout (Lepage et al., 2005a). While social aggression between trout does raise plasma melatonin, the results suggest that altered production contributes to the physiological and behavioral profiles of subordinate rather than aggressive fish (Larson et al., 2004). Even in hamsters, aggression stimulated by increased melatonin appears to be mediated by glucocorticoids (Demas et al., 2004). What is more, treatment with the SSRI, citalopram, for 7 days closely mimicked the effects of Trp (Lepage et al., 2005a). These results suggest that the effects of elevated dietary intake of Trp on aggressive behavior are mediated by the serotonergic system even though the effects on brain 5-HT turnover, as indicated by brain levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) and 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios (Lepage et al., 2003; Lepage et al., 2002; Winberg et al., 2001), are relatively modest. In fact, in the study by Winberg et al. (Winberg et al., 2001) the lowest dose of Trp applied had no significant effects on brain 5-HIAA levels or 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios even though the effects on aggressive behavior were pronounced. Thus, if the brain serotonergic system is mediating the effects of elevated dietary Trp on aggressive behavior mechanisms other than increased 5-HT biosynthesis and release are likely to be involved. It is noticeable that the anti-depressive effects of SSRI show a time course strikingly similar to the anti-aggressive effects of Trp; although the effects of SSRI on aggressive behavior can be as rapid as a single treatment (Perreault et al., 2003; Taravosh-Lahn et al., 2006). Also, similar to elevated dietary Trp, a 1-week treatment with citalopram reduces aggressive behavior in rainbow trout (Lepage et al., 2005a), as does sertraline treatment in Anolis lizards (Larson and Summers, 2001). Long-term elevation of 5-HT release (stage 4), whether induced by chronic stress, elevated dietary Trp, or SSRI treatment, could well cause a downregulation of somatodendritic 5-HT autoreceptors, in turn resulting in a delayed upregulation of 5-HT neurotransmission in terminal fields (Mongeau et al., 1997). The anti-depressive effects of SSRIs have been suggested to depend on actions influencing densities and transduction mechanisms of post- and/or pre-synaptic 5-HT receptors, resulting in such a delayed increase of 5-HT postsynaptic effects in specific brain regions. It is tempting to suggest that the effects of dietary Trp and SSRI on aggressive behavior in rainbow trout are mediated by a similar mechanism. In fact, preliminary results suggest a downregulation of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA expression in the raphé area of rainbow trout fed Trp-supplemented feed for 1 week (P.-O. Thörnqvist, O. Lepage and S. Winberg, unpublished results).

Even though the long-term stress-induced elevation in brain serotonergic activity (during stages 1 or 4) is likely to be an important factor mediating behavioral inhibition in subordinate animals, other neurotransmitter systems are most likely to also be involved. Interestingly, Trp-supplemented feed, as well as SSRI, lowers the number of attacks performed against a small conspecific intruder but neither has any effect on the attack latencies (Lepage et al., 2005a). Social subordination, on the other hand, results in a lower number of attacks performed against the intruder as well as longer attack latencies (Lepage et al., 2005a). Thus, rainbow trout in which brain serotonergic activity has been stimulated by means of elevated dietary Trp or SSRI treatment attack the intruder as fast as control fish, but in fish treated with Trp or SSRI, aggression does not escalate to the same level as in control fish.

Dopamine and motivation

Although high levels of 5-HT in specific regions of aggression neurocircuitry apparently can inhibit agonistic interaction before it begins (stage 1) (Summers et al., 2005b), under specific ethological or environmental conditions this tonic inhibition must be overridden. Elevated motivation for aggression (stage 2) appears to be the province of dopaminergic action, and aggressive social interaction stimulates dopaminergic as well as serotonergic activity in limbic brain regions. The limbic functions of dopamine (DA) seem to include integrative regulation of a variety of related activities including aggression (Höglund et al., 2005; Kramarcy et al., 1984), social status (Winberg and Nilsson, 1992), motor activity (Waters et al., 2005), learning and perhaps most important, motivation and reward (Korzan et al., 2006a). All of these parameters influence social interaction, and are integrated as a part of neural regulation of social stress. During social interactions between male A. carolinensis, increased hypothalamic DA and increased DA in substantia nigra and ventral tegmental areas (SN/VTA) were associated with increased aggressive behavior and status (Korzan et al., 2006a). Elevated dopaminergic activity is usually associated with greater aggressiveness and elevated social status, but very high levels may even limit aggressive interaction (Höglund et al., 2005).

In Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus, dominant fish show elevated brain dopaminergic activity (Winberg et al., 1991) and l-3,4-dihydroxyphenylalanine (l-DOPA) treatment, which elevates DA activity, increases the chance of fish to become dominant in dyadic fight for social dominance with a size matched conspecific (Winberg and Nilsson, 1992). Moreover, in this species l-DOPA treatment counteracts the stress-induced elevation of brain 5-HT activity and plasma cortisol concentrations (Höglund et al., 2001).

Like most vertebrates, locomotor activity stimulates increased DA in striatum of lizards (Waters et al., 2005). However, during aggressive displays and attacks, changes observed in dopaminergic activity in nuclei associated with motor activity like striatum seem to be coupled with expression of specific stereotyped movements associated with social communication more than general motor activity (Korzan et al., 2006a; Baxter, 2003; Baxter et al., 2001a; Baxter, 2001; Clark et al., 2000). The striatum and nucleus accumbens are closely linked, as are level of motivation and stereotyped social behavior.

Increases in DA and dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) in the nucleus accumbens occur when males successfully achieve dominant status and may be representative of motivation and reward. Effects of social interaction on dopaminergic activity in hippocampal and amygdalar nuclei appear consistent with current literature on neurochemical changes associated with memory formation. Together it suggests that the combination of social signal perception, social rank and behavioral expression, but not any single factor (Summers et al., 2005a), may be the impetus for the changes in dopaminergic activity associated with formation of dominant and subordinate status.

Glucocorticoid influence on aggression

The adrenocortical or interrenal steroid hormones corticosterone and cortisol influence aggression and serotonergic activity (Summers et al., 2005c). The influence of glucocorticoids on serotonergic function is much easier to summarize. Glucocorticoids increase serotonergic function, both acutely and chronically (Summers et al., 2003b; Summers et al., 2000), the latter primarily through increased activity of the synthetic enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase (TpH) (see Summers et al., 2005b). However, the effects of glucocorticoids on aggressive behavior, like that of 5-HT, is much more complex, and depends on context and timing (Summers et al., 2005c). The simplest description is that chronically elevated glucocorticoids generally inhibit aggression, but acutely applied corticosteroids do not. In fact, glucocorticoids applied acutely to mammals (rodents at least), especially applied directly to the anterior hypothalamus, stimulate aggressive behavior (Hayden-Hixson and Ferris, 1991; Kruk et al., 2004). In addition, chronic glucocorticoid deficiency promotes abnormal, even pathological, aggressive attack in rats (Haller and Kruk, 2006; Haller et al., 2004; Haller et al., 2001). In other vertebrates, short-term elevation of glucocorticoids appears to passively allow aggressive behavior (see Summers et al., 2005c), but that is not the entire story.

Among lizards, the stress of aggressive interaction stimulates a rapid increase in corticosterone for both winners and losers (Summers et al., 2005c), as well as males and females (Woodley et al., 2000); with the type and duration of interaction determining the manner of glucocorticoid secretion (Knapp and Moore, 1995). A video image of an aggressive Anolis carolinensis is enough stimulus to elicit corticosterone secretion in a conspecific (Yang and Wilczynski, 2003). In the lizard A. carolinensis, while acute corticosterone administration does not affect aggressive interaction between two males, acute administration of the glucocorticoid receptor blocker mifepristone (RU486) does (Summers et al., 2005c). Blocking glucocorticoid receptors reduces aggressive attacks and displays during the early critical portion (first 7 min) of aggressive behavior, but not later. This suggests that glucocorticoids are necessary for aggressive behavior, if only permissively. In addition, putatively dominant, precipitously active [or proactive as per (Koolhaas et al., 1999)] male A. carolinensis have significantly higher baseline glucocorticoid concentrations prior to any social interaction (Summers et al., 2005b).

In rainbow trout, short-term treatment with cortisol increases activity of fish challenged by a conspecific intruder whereas long-term treatment with cortisol has the opposite effect, inhibiting the increase in activity induced by the presence of the intruder (Øverli et al., 2002). However, cortisol had no effects on locomotor activity in rainbow trout not challenged by an intruder. The effects of cortisol on aggressive behavior show a similar pattern, long-term treatment inhibiting aggression in rainbow trout whereas short-term treatment appears to have the opposite effect (Øverli et al., 2002). Thus, cortisol has dose- and context-dependent effects on behavioral responsiveness in rainbow trout, suggesting that cortisol could be a factor contributing to the divergent behavioral effects of short- and long-term social stress, long-term social stress resulting in an obvious behavioral inhibition whereas short-term social stress may have the opposite effect (Øverli et al., 2002).

Serotonergic modulation of stress responsiveness

Aggressive interaction is stressful for both the winner and the loser (Summers et al., 2005a; Summers et al., 2005c; Øverli et al., 1999). Therefore, it is not surprising that neural and hormonal indices of stress responsiveness are elevated for all participants of combative social interaction during aggression. Inasmuch as acute serotonergic and glucocorticoid responses during aggression are not inhibiting active and combative social interaction (Summers et al., 2005b; Summers et al., 2005c), the function of elevated central serotonergic activity and plasma corticosteroid concentrations appears to be associated with stress (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993).

Neural and hormonal stress responses that occur during aggressive interaction are very rapid (Summers, 2001; Summers et al., 2005c). Serotonergic activity in the brain is elevated within 30 s in dominant territorial males (Matter et al., 1998). What is more, by 90 s, concomitant with elevated serotonergic activity in brain regions like nucleus accumbens, plasma glucocorticoid levels are also increased, at least in dominant males (Summers et al., 2005c). In addition, both the serotonergic and glucocorticoid responses are similar in timing and magnitude in response to physical stressors like exercise and restraint as in response to social stress (Emerson et al., 2000; Summers et al., 2005c).

The serotonergic system is generally believed to stimulate hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal/interrenal (HPA/HPI) axis activity, and brain 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios have been found to correlate with plasma levels of cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in rainbow trout and Arctic charr (Øverli et al., 1999; Höglund et al., 2000; Winberg and Lepage, 1998). However, inhibitory effects of 5-HT on the glucocorticoid response have also been reported. For instance, in salmonid fish, the selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist, 8-hydroxy-2-(di-N-propylamino) tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) may have either stimulatory or inhibitory effects on HPI axis activity depending on dose and context. In undisturbed fish 8-OH-DPAT stimulates HPI axis activity (Höglund et al., 2002; Winberg et al., 1997). By contrast, if administered at low doses to stressed Arctic charr, 8-OH-DPAT has the opposite effect, suppressing the stress-induced elevation of plasma ACTH and cortisol (Höglund et al., 2002). Similarly, stimulation of brain 5-HT activity by elevated dietary levels of Trp results in slightly elevated basal plasma levels of cortisol, but at the same time the stress-induced elevation of plasma ACTH and cortisol concentrations is drastically reduced (Lepage et al., 2003; Lepage et al., 2002). The effect of dietary Trp on HPI axis reactivity follows the same time-course as the effects on aggressive behavior (discussed above) (Lepage et al., 2003). Moreover, the effects of Trp on HPI axis reactivity cannot be explained by effects on brain catecholaminergic systems or plasma melatonin (Lepage et al., 2005a; Lepage et al., 2003). Thus, Trp effects on HPI axis reactivity is most likely mediated by the brain 5-HT system, but not necessarily by direct effects on 5-HT synthesis and release, since such effects would be more rapid than the effects observed on HPI axis reactivity. Instead, effects on pre- and postsynaptic 5-HT receptor densities are more likely as a mechanism mediating these effects of dietary Trp on the glucocorticoid response.

A timeline for social stress

Socially interacting individuals perceive, generate, and integrate various relevant sensory stimuli during an interaction. For male A. carolinensis that includes visual stimuli such as eyespots and stereotyped behavior. However, even before social interactions occur (Fig. 1), the neuroendocrine status of an individual (stage 1), perhaps influenced by previous interactions (stage 4), can influence the rate and probability of the behavioral responses (Summers et al., 2005b). Tonic neuroendocrine status sets the stage for behavioral tone (stage 1), which is primarily inhibitory regulation of aggressive behavior. High serotonergic activity in reactive submissive subordinate males, and those that will become so appears to inhibit aggression. On the other hand, in putatively proactive (Koolhaas et al., 1999) and aggressive dominant males, tonically low 5-HT release lowers the threshold, making aggression much more rapid and likely. Neuroendocrine tone prior to aggressive interaction may be subsequently influenced by motivational factors (stage 2), including perception of a combatant and his sign stimuli, which corresponds to changes in dopaminergic activity in specific brain nuclei. Brief presentation of a simulated opponent is effective in evoking rapid changes in neuroendocrine activity. While sympathetically mediated plasma catecholamine levels were no different from increases elicited by environmental disturbance, social challenge caused distinct changes in accumbal and amygdalar DA and 5-HT levels (Watt et al., 2006). Changes in the amygdala are associated with general social threat presence. In concert with amygdalar activity, the level of perceived social threat, individual variation in threat level assessment, and motivation to convey aggressiveness, all affect monoamine levels in the nucleus accumbens. This suggests that perception of a non-displaying opponent is sufficient both for recognition of social context and for inducing rapid activity changes in key forebrain limbic nuclei to initiate appropriate behavioral responses. The latency to aggressive behavior, attack and darkening of the eyespot sign stimuli are most rapid in those that become dominant. However, regardless of level of aggressive behavioral expression and individual social status, once agonistic interactions begin, stress related elevation in serotonergic activity in aggression neurocircuitry and glucocorticoids in plasma occurs rapidly. For a short period, aggressive behavior is elevated in both dominant and subordinate animals, despite high serotonergic and glucocorticoid activity (stage 3). However, the stress response of dominant males to this aggressive interaction is relatively short lived, and serotonergic activity along with glucocorticoid concentrations return to normal. For subordinate males, chronically elevated glucocorticoid (Greenberg et al., 1984) and serotonergic activity (stage 4) limits aggressive behavior while they are in contact with the dominant male, and again if they are reintroduced to the same dominant male. Presumably, these chronically elevated stress chemicals may be reflected in the elevated serotonergic activity measured before aggressive interaction (stage 1). However, subordinate males actually are more aggressive in a second aggressive bout when the dominant male that he is fighting is unknown to him (Forster et al., 2005). Whether elevated glucocorticoids plays a role in enhanced aggression towards an unknown opponent remains to be tested. Previous interactions are known to influence aggressive responses, even if it simply involves watching aggression between other individuals (Oliveira et al., 2001; Oliveira et al., 1998). In addition, bullied individuals react more aggressively toward a different smaller opponent (Øverli et al., 2004).

Conclusion

The central neurotransmitter serotonin and the adrenocortical/interrenal steroid hormones corticosterone and cortisol are important for both stress and aggression. While much of the neurocircuitry for stress and aggression overlap, the timing of responsiveness for 5-HT and glucocorticoids does not. That is, 5-HT and glucocorticoids can both be inhibitory to aggression, but are not inhibitory during the early stressful phase of aggression. This transmitter-hormone combination follows a linear pattern of effect: (1) influencing predisposition (positively or negatively) toward aggression, (2) influencing motivation toward behavior (3) responsive to stress (including aggression) and passively allowing aggression, and finally (4) chronically applied 5-HT and glucocorticoids inhibit aggression. The pattern of neurochemical and hormonal events stimulated by social interaction make it clear that socially aggressive interaction is stressful. Although social aggression has been demonstrated to be stressful for both dominant and subordinate animals, subtle differences in the pattern of response distinguish social rank. Understanding the mechanisms that promote those subtle distinctions are necessary to understand the relationship between stress and aggression more clearly.

List of abbreviations
5-HIAA
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid
5-HT
serotonin
8-OH-DPAT
8-hydroxy-2-(di-N-propylamino) tetralin
ACTH
adrenocorticotropic hormone
AVP
vasopressin
BNST
bed nucleus of the stria terminalis
DA
dopamine
DOPAC
dihydroxyphenylacetic acid
GABAA
γ-aminobutyric acid type A
Glu
glutamate
HPA/HPI
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal/interrenal axis
l-DOPA
l-3,4-dihydroxyphenylalanine
NE
norepinephrine
PAG
periaqueductal gray
SN/VTA
substantia nigra and ventral tegmental areas
SSRI
selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitors
TpH
tryptophan hydroxylase
Trp
l-tryptophan

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Work in the Winberg lab is supported by the Swedish Council for Environmental, Agricultural Sciences and Spatial Planning (FORMAS) and the European Commission. In the Summers' lab our work is supported by NIH grants P20 RR15567, R03 MH068303 & R01 DA019921 (to G.L.F.), R03 MH068364 (to M.J.W.), 1 F31 MH64983 (to W.J.K.). We would like to thank Tom Andrews, Tobias Backström, Daniel Brelin, Russ Carpenter, Aaron Emerson, Gina Forster (G.L.F.), Teresa Grant, Neil Greenberg, Erik Höglund, Nicklas Kolm, Wayne Korzan (W.J.K.), Earl Larson, Olivier Lepage, Travis Ling, Jodi Lukkes, Ian Mayer, John Matter, Jen McKay, Whitney Meyer, Göran E. Nilsson,Ø yvind Øverli, Aaron Prestbo, Ken Renner, Pat Ronan, Joachim Schjolden, Tangi Summers, Gary Ten Eyck, Per-Ove Thörnqvist, Olof Tottmar, and Michael Watt (M.J.W.) for collecting the data, and their tireless work and creative ideas.

  • © The Company of Biologists Limited 2006

References

  1. ↵
    Aldegunde, M., Garcia, J., Soengas, J. L. and Rozas, G. (1998). Uptake of tryptophan into brain of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). J. Exp. Zool. 282,285 -290.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    Aldegunde, M., Soengas, J. L. and Rozas, G. (2000). Acute effects of L-tryptophan on tryptophan hydroxylation rate in brain regions (hypothalamus and medulla) of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). J. Exp. Zool. 286,131 -135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Azrin, N. H., Hutchison, R. R. and Hake, D. F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 9,191 -204.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  4. ↵
    Baxter, L. R. (2001). Brain mediation of Anolis social dominance displays. III. Differential forebrain 3H-sumatriptan binding in dominant vs. submissive males. Brain Behav. Evol. 57,202 -213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Baxter, L. R. (2003). Basal ganglia systems in ritualistic social displays: reptiles and humans; function and illness. Physiol. Behav. 79,451 -460.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Baxter, L. R., Ackermann, R. F., Clark, E. C. and Baxter, J. E. (2001a). Brain mediation of Anolis social dominance displays. I. Differential basal ganglia activation. Brain Behav. Evol. 57,169 -183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    Baxter, L. R., Clark, E. C., Ackermann, R. F., Lacan, G. and Melega, W. P. (2001b). Brain mediation of Anolis social dominance displays. II. Differential forebrain serotonin turnover, and effects of specific 5-HT receptor agonists. Brain Behav. Evol. 57,184 -201.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. ↵
    Brody, J. F., DeFeudis, P. A. and DeFeudis, F. V. (1969). Effects of micro-injections of L-glutamate into the hypothalamus on attack and flight behaviour in cats. Nature 224,1330 .
    OpenUrlPubMed
  9. ↵
    Cherek, D. R. and Pickens, R. (1970). Schedule-induced aggression as a function of fixed-ratio value. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 14,309 -311.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  10. ↵
    Clark, E. C., Baxter, L. R., Dure, L. S., Ackermann, R. F., Kemp, G. F. and Bachus, S. E. (2000). Mammal-like striatal functions in Anolis. II. Distribution of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors, and a laminar pattern of basal ganglia sub-systems. Brain Behav. Evol. 56,249 -258.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    David, J. T., Cervantes, M. C., Trosky, K. A., Salinas, J. A. and Delville, Y. (2004). A neural network underlying individual differences in emotion and aggression in male golden hamsters. Neuroscience 126,567 -578.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  12. ↵
    de Almeida, R. M., Ferrari, P. F., Parmigiani, S. and Miczek, K. A. (2005). Escalated aggressive behavior: dopamine, serotonin and GABA. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526, 51-64.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    de Boer, S. F. and Koolhaas, J. M. (2005). 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor agonists and aggression: a pharmacological challenge of the serotonin deficiency hypothesis. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526,125 -139.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  14. ↵
    Deckel, A. W. (1996). Behavioral changes in Anolis carolinensis following injection with fluoxetine. Behav. Brain Res. 78,175 -182.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  15. ↵
    Deckel, A. W. and Fuqua, L. (1998). Effects of serotonergic drugs on lateralized aggression and aggressive displays in Anolis carolinensis. Behav. Brain Res. 95,227 -232.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    Delville, Y., De Vries, G. J. and Ferris, C. F. (2000). Neural connections of the anterior hypothalamus and agonistic behavior in golden hamsters. Brain Behav. Evol. 55,53 -76.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  17. ↵
    Delville, Y., David, J. T., Taravosh-Lahn, K. and Wommack, J. C. (2003). Stress and the development of agonistic behavior in golden hamsters. Horm. Behav. 44,263 -270.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    Demas, G. E., Polacek, K. M., Durazzo, A. and Jasnow, A. M. (2004). Adrenal hormones mediate melatonin-induced increases in aggression in male Siberian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus). Horm. Behav. 46,582 -591.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Emerson, A. J., Kappenman, D. P., Ronan, P. J., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2000). Stress induces rapid changes in serotonergic activity: restraint and exertion. Behav. Brain Res. 111,83 -92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  20. ↵
    Ferrari, P. F., Palanza, P., Parmigiani, S., de Alimeida, R. M. and Miczek, K. A. (2005). Serotonin and aggressive behavior in rodents and nonhuman primates: predispositions and plasticity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526,259 -273.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Ferris, C. F. and Delville, Y. (1994). Vasopressin and serotonin interactions in the control of agonistic behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19,593 -601.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  22. ↵
    Ferris, C. F., Melloni, R. H., Jr, Koppel, G., Perry, K. W., Fuller, R. W. and Delville, Y. (1997). Vasopressin/serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden hamsters. J. Neurosci. 17,4331 -4340.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    Ferris, C. F., Stolberg, T. and Delville, Y. (1999). Serotonin regulation of aggressive behavior in male golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 113,804 -815.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    Forster, G. L., Watt, M. J., Korzan, W. J., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2005). Opponent recognition in male green anoles, Anolis carolinensis. Anim. Behav. 69,733 -740.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    Friedman, P. A., Kappelman, A. H. and Kaufman, S. (1972). Partial purification and characterization of tryptophan hydroxylase from rabbit hindbrain. J. Biol. Chem. 247,4165 -4173.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Gallup, G. G. (1965). Aggression in rats as a function of frustrative nonreward in a straight alley. Psychon. Sci. 3,99 -100.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    Greenberg, N., Chen, T. and Crews, D. (1984). Social status, gonadal state, and the adrenal stress response in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 18, 1-11.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  28. ↵
    Gregg, T. R. and Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: implications for human aggression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 25,91 -140.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Halasz, J., Liposits, Z., Meelis, W., Kruk, M. R. and Haller, J. (2002). Hypothalamic attack area-mediated activation of the forebrain in aggression. NeuroReport 13,1267 -1270.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. ↵
    Haller, J. and Kruk, M. R. (2006). Normal and abnormal aggression: human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30,292 -303.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    Haller, J., van de Schraaf, J. and Kruk, M. R. (2001). Deviant forms of aggression in glucocorticoid hyporeactive rats: a model for `pathological' aggression? J. Neuroendocrinol. 13,102 -107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Haller, J., Halasz, J., Mikics, E. and Kruk, M. R. (2004). Chronic glucocorticoid deficiency-induced abnormal aggression, autonomic hypoarousal, and social deficit in rats. J. Neuroendocrinol. 16,550 -557.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. ↵
    Hayden-Hixson, D. M. and Ferris, C. F. (1991). Steroid-specific regulation of agonistic responding in the anterior hypothalamus of male hamsters. Physiol. Behav. 50,793 -799.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Höglund, E., Balm, P. H. and Winberg, S. (2000). Skin darkening, a potential social signal in subordinate arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus): the regulatory role of brain monoamines and pro-opiomelanocortin-derived peptides. J. Exp. Biol. 203,1711 -1721.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  35. ↵
    Höglund, E., Kolm, N. and Winberg, S. (2001). Stress-induced changes in brain serotonergic activity, plasma cortisol and aggressive behavior in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) is counteracted by L-DOPA. Physiol. Behav. 74,381 -389.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Höglund, E., Balm, P. H. and Winberg, S. (2002). Stimulatory and inhibitory effects of 5-HT1A receptors on adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol secretion in a teleost fish, the Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus). Neurosci. Lett. 324,193 -196.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  37. ↵
    Höglund, E., Korzan, W. J., Watt, M. J., Forster, G. L., Summers, T. R., Johannessen, H. F., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2005). Effects of L-DOPA on aggressive behavior and central monoaminergic activity in the lizard Anolis carolinensis, using a new method for drug delivery. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 53-64.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Hrabovszky, E., Halasz, J., Meelis, W., Kruk, M. R., Liposits, Z. and Haller, J. (2005). Neurochemical characterization of hypothalamic neurons involved in attack behavior: Glutamatergic dominance and co-expression of thyrotropin-releasing hormone in a subset of glutamatergic neurons. Neuroscience 133,657 -666.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Jasnow, A. M., Huhman, K. L., Bartness, T. J. and Demas, G. E. (2002). Short days and exogenous melatonin increase aggression of male Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Horm. Behav. 42,13 -20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. ↵
    Johnston, W. L., Atkinson, J. L., Hilton, J. W. and Were, K. E. (1990). Effect of dietary tryptophan on plasma and brain tryptophan, brain serotonin, and brain 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in rainbow trout. J. Nutr. Biochem. 1, 49-54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Knapp, R. and Moore, M. C. (1995). Hormonal responses to aggression vary in different types of agonistic encounters in male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Horm. Behav. 29, 85-105.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., de Boer, S. F., van der Vegt, B. J., van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., de Jong, I. C., Ruis, M. A. and Blokhuis, H. J. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23,925 -935.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  43. ↵
    Korzan, W. J., Forster, G. L., Watt, M. J. and Summers, C. H. (2006a). Dopaminergic activity modulation via aggression, status and a visual social signal. Behav. Neurosci. 120,93 -102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  44. ↵
    Korzan, W. J., Øverli, Ø. and Summers, C. H. (2006b). Future social rank: forecasting status in the green anole (Anolis carolinensis). Acta Ethol. 9, 48-57.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    Kramarcy, N. R., Brown, J. W. and Thurmond, J. B. (1984). Effects of drug-induced changes in brain monoamines on aggression and motor behavior in mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 99,141 -151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Kruk, M. R., van der Poel, A. M. and Vos-Frerichs, T. P. (1979). The induction of aggressive behaviour by electrical stimulation in the hypothalamus of male rats. Behaviour 70,292 -322.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    Kruk, M. R., Halász, J., Meelis, W. and Haller, J. (2004). Fast positive feedback between the adrenocortical stress response and a brain mechanism involved in aggressive behavior. Behav. Neurosci. 118,1062 -1070.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. ↵
    Larson, E. T. and Summers, C. H. (2001). Serotonin reverses dominant social status. Behav. Brain Res. 121,95 -102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. ↵
    Larson, E. T., Norris, D. O. and Summers, C. H. (2003). Monoaminergic changes associated with socially induced sex reversal in the saddleback wrasse. Neuroscience 119,251 -263.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Larson, E. T., Winberg, S., Mayer, I., Lepage, O., Summers, C. H. and Overli, O. (2004). Social stress affects circulating melatonin levels in rainbow trout. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 136,322 -327.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Lepage, O., Tottmar, O. and Winberg, S. (2002). Elevated dietary intake of L-tryptophan counteracts the stress-induced elevation of plasma cortisol in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). J. Exp. Biol. 205,3679 -3687.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Lepage, O., Vilchez, I. M., Pottinger, T. G. and Winberg, S. (2003). Time-course of the effect of dietary L-tryptophan on plasma cortisol levels in rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss. J. Exp. Biol. 206,3589 -3599.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    Lepage, O., Larson, E. T., Mayer, I. and Winberg, S. (2005a). Serotonin, but not melatonin, plays a role in shaping dominant-subordinate relationships and aggression in rainbow trout. Horm. Behav. 48,233 -242.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Lepage, O., Larson, E. T., Mayer, I. and Winberg, S. (2005b). Tryptophan affects both gastrointestinal melatonin production and interrenal activity in stressed and nonstressed rainbow trout. J. Pineal Res. 38,264 -271.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Matter, J. M., Ronan, P. J. and Summers, C. H. (1998). Central monoamines in free-ranging lizards: differences associated with social roles and territoriality. Brain Behav. Evol. 51,23 -32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  56. ↵
    Mitchell, P. J. (2005). Antidepressant treatment and rodent aggressive behaviour. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526,147 -162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Mitchell, P. J. and Redfern, P. H. (1992). Acute and chronic antidepressant drug treatments induce opposite effects in the social behaviour of rats. J. Psychopharmacol. 6, 241-257.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Mitchell, P. J. and Redfern, P. H. (1997). Potentiation of the time-dependent, antidepressant-induced changes in the agonistic behaviour of resident rats by the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, WAY-100635. Behav. Pharmacol. 8, 585-606.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Moir, A. T. and Eccleston, D. (1968). The effects of precursor loading in the cerebral metabolism of 5-hydroxyindoles. J. Neurochem. 15,1093 -1108.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  60. ↵
    Mongeau, R., Blier, P. and de Montigny, C. (1997). The serotonergic and noradrenergic systems of the hippocampus: their interactions and the effects of antidepressant treatments. Brain Res. Rev. 23,145 -195.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Nelson, R. J. and Chiavegatto, S. (2001). Molecular basis of aggression. Trends Neurosci. 24,713 -719.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. ↵
    Oliveira, R. F., McGregor, P. K. and Latruffe, C. (1998). Know thine enemy: fighting fish gather information from observing conspecific interactions. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 265,1045 -1049.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    Oliveira, R. F., Lopes, M., Carneiro, L. A. and Canario, A. V. (2001). Watching fights raises fish hormone levels. Nature 409,475 .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Øverli, Ø., Harris, C. A. and Winberg, S. (1999). Short-term effects of fights for social dominance and the establishment of dominant-subordinate relationships on brain monoamines and cortisol in rainbow trout. Brain Behav. Evol. 54,263 -275.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  65. ↵
    Øverli, Ø., Kotzian, S. and Winberg, S. (2002). Effects of cortisol on aggression and locomotor activity in rainbow trout. Horm. Behav. 42, 53-61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  66. ↵
    Øverli, Ø., Korzan, W. J., Larson, E. T., Winberg, S., Lepage, O., Pottinger, T. G., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2004). Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of displaced aggression in trout. Horm. Behav. 45,324 -329.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Perreault, H. A., Semsar, K. and Godwin, J. (2003). Fluoxetine treatment decreases territorial aggression in a coral reef fish. Physiol. Behav. 79,719 -724.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    Ricci, L. A., Grimes, J. M. and Melloni, R. H. (2004). Serotonin type 3 receptors modulate the aggression-stimulating effects of adolescent cocaine exposure in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 118,1097 -1110.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    Ricci, L. A., Knyshevski, I. and Melloni, R. H., Jr (2005). Serotonin type 3 receptors stimulate offensive aggression in Syrian hamsters. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 19-29.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  70. ↵
    Roeling, T. A., Veening, J. G., Kruk, M. R., Peters, J. P., Vermelis, M. E. and Nieuwenhuys, R. (1994). Efferent connections of the hypothalamic `aggression area' in the rat. Neuroscience 59,1001 -1024.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. ↵
    Siegel, A., Roeling, T. A., Gregg, T. R. and Kruk, M. R. (1999). Neuropharmacology of brain-stimulation-evoked aggression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23,359 -389.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. ↵
    Summers, C. H. (2001). Mechanisms for quick and variable responses. Brain Behav. Evol. 57,283 -292.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  73. ↵
    Summers, C. H. (2002). Social interaction over time, implications for stress responsiveness. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42,591 -599.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    Summers, C. H., Larson, E. T., Ronan, P. J., Hofmann, P. M., Emerson, A. J. and Renner, K. J. (2000). Serotonergic responses to corticosterone and testosterone in the limbic system. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 117,151 -159.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  75. ↵
    Summers, C. H., Summers, T. R., Moore, M. C., Korzan, W. J., Woodley, S. K., Ronan, P. J., Höglund, E., Watt, M. J. and Greenberg, N. (2003a). Temporal patterns of limbic monoamine and plasma corticosterone response during social stress. Neuroscience 116,553 -563.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  76. ↵
    Summers, T. R., Matter, J. M., McKay, J. M., Ronan, P. J., Larson, E. T., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2003b). Rapid glucocorticoid stimulation and GABAergic inhibition of hippocampal serotonergic response: in vivo dialysis in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 43,245 -253.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Summers, C. H., Forster, G. L., Korzan, W. J., Watt, M. J., Larson, E. T., Overli, O., Hoglund, E., Ronan, P. J., Summers, T. R., Renner, K. J. et al. (2005a). Dynamics and mechanics of social rank reversal. J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 191,241 -252.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    Summers, C. H., Korzan, W. J., Lukkes, J. L., Øverli,Ø ., Höglund, E., Watt, M. J., Larson, E. T., Forster, G. L., Ronan, P. J., Summers, T. R. et al. (2005b). Does serotonin influence aggression? Comparing regional activity before and during social interaction. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 78,679 -694.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  79. ↵
    Summers, C. H., Watt, M. J., Ling, T. J., Forster, G. L., Carpenter, R. E., Korzan, W. J., Lukkes, J. L. and Øverli,Ø . (2005c). Glucocorticoid interaction with aggression in non-mammalian vertebrates: reciprocal action. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526,21 -35.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    Taravosh-Lahn, K., Bastida, C. and Delville, Y. (2006). Differential responsiveness to fluoxetine during puberty. Behav. Neurosci. 120,1084 -1092.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    van Erp, A. M. and Miczek, K. A. (2000). Aggressive behavior, increased accumbal dopamine, and decreased cortical serotonin in rats. J. Neurosci. 20,9320 -9325.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  82. ↵
    Vergnes, M., Depaulis, A., Boehrer, A. and Kempf, E. (1988). Selective increase of offensive behavior in the rat following intrahypothalamic 5,7-DHT-induced serotonin depletion. Behav. Brain Res. 29,85 -91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  83. ↵
    Waters, R. P., Emerson, A. J., Watt, M. J., Forster, G. L., Swallow, J. G. and Summers, C. H. (2005). Stress induces rapid changes in central catecholaminergic activity in Anolis carolinensis: restraint and forced physical activity. Brain Res. Bull. 67,210 -218.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    Watt, M. J., Forster, G. L., Korzan, W. J., Renner, K. J. and Summers, C. H. (2006). Initial perception of social threat rapidly alters neuroendocrine activity. Physiol. Behav. In Press.
  85. ↵
    Winberg, S. and Lepage, O. (1998). Elevation of brain 5-HT activity, POMC expression, and plasma cortisol in socially subordinate rainbow trout. Am. J. Physiol. 274,R645 -R654.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  86. ↵
    Winberg, S. and Nilsson, G. E. (1992). Induction of social dominance by L-dopa treatment in Arctic charr. NeuroReport 3,243 -246.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  87. ↵
    Winberg, S. and Nilsson, G. E. (1993). Roles of brain monoamine transmitters in agonistic behaviour and stress reactions, with particular reference to fish. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 106C,597 -614.
    OpenUrl
  88. ↵
    Winberg, S., Nilsson, G. E. and Olsén, K. H. (1991). Social rank and brain levels of monoamines and monoamine metabolites in Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus (L.). J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 168,241 -246.
    OpenUrl
  89. ↵
    Winberg, S., Nilsson, A., Hylland, P., Soderstom, V. and Nilsson, G. E. (1997). Serotonin as a regulator of hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal activity in teleost fish. Neurosci. Lett. 230,113 -116.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  90. ↵
    Winberg, S., Øverli, Ø. and Lepage, O. (2001). Suppression of aggression in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) by dietary L-tryptophan. J. Exp. Biol. 204,3867 -3876.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  91. ↵
    Wommack, J. C., Taravosh-Lahn, K., David, J. T. and Delville, Y. (2003). Repeated exposure to social stress alters the development of agonistic behavior in male golden hamsters. Horm. Behav. 43,229 -236.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    Wommack, J. C., Salinas, A., Melloni, R. H., Jr and Delville, Y. (2004). Behavioural and neuroendocrine adaptations to repeated stress during puberty in male golden hamsters. J. Neuroendocrinol. 16,767 -775.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    Woodley, S. K., Matt, K. S. and Moore, M. C. (2000). Neuroendocrine responses in free-living female and male lizards after aggressive interactions. Physiol. Behav. 71,373 -381.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    Yang, E. J. and Wilczynski, W. (2003). Interaction effects of corticosterone and experience on aggressive behavior in the green anole lizard. Horm. Behav. 44,281 -292.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    Yao, R., Rameshwar, P., Gregg, T. and Siegel, A. (2001). Co-localization of NK1-receptor mRNA with glutamate immunopositivity in cat hypothalamic neurons by the combination of in situ hybridization and immunohistochemistry. Brain Res. Brain Res. Protoc. 7,154 -161.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Commentary
Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression
Cliff H. Summers, Svante Winberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2006 209: 4581-4589; doi: 10.1242/jeb.02565
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Commentary
Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression
Cliff H. Summers, Svante Winberg
Journal of Experimental Biology 2006 209: 4581-4589; doi: 10.1242/jeb.02565

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • Introduction
    • Aggression neurocircuitry
    • Serotonergic inhibition of aggression
    • Dopamine and motivation
    • Glucocorticoid influence on aggression
    • Serotonergic modulation of stress responsiveness
    • A timeline for social stress
    • Conclusion
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Thermal robustness of biomechanical processes
  • Help, there are ‘omics’ in my comparative physiology!
  • Structural plasticity of the avian pectoralis: a case for geometry and the forgotten organelle
Show more COMMENTARY

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


Critical temperature window sends migratory black-headed buntings on their travels

The spring rise in temperature at black-headed bunting overwintering sites is essential for triggering the physical changes that they undergo before embarking on their spring migration – read more.


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992