Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Research Article
The tremble dance of honey bees can be caused by hive-external foraging experience
Corinna Thom
Journal of Experimental Biology 2003 206: 2111-2116; doi: 10.1242/jeb.00398
Corinna Thom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

SUMMARY

The tremble dance of honey bee nectar foragers is part of the communication system that regulates a colony's foraging efficiency. A forager that returns to the hive with nectar, but then experiences a long unloading delay because she has difficulty finding a nectar receiver bee, will perform a tremble dance to recruit additional nectar receiver bees. A forager that experiences a short unloading delay will perform a waggle dance to recruit more nectar foragers. A long unloading delay was until now the only known cause of tremble dancing. However, several studies suggested that factors at the food source may also cause tremble dancing. Here I test whether one of these factors, crowding of nectar foragers at the food source, stimulates tremble dancing because it causes long unloading delays. To do so, I increased the density of nectar foragers at a food source by suddenly reducing the size of an artificial feeder, and recorded the unloading delay experienced by each forager, as well as the dance she performed, if any. A forager's unloading delay was measured as the time interval between entering the hive and either (1) the first unloading contact with a nectar receiver bee, or (2) the start of the first dance, if dancing began before the first unloading contact. I also recorded the unloading delays and dances of nectar foragers that returned from natural food sources. The results show that crowding of nectar foragers at the food source increases the probability of tremble dancing, but does not cause long unloading delays, and that tremble dancers that foraged at natural food sources also often have short unloading delays. When the cause of the tremble dance is not a low supply of nectar receiver bees, the tremble dance may have a function in addition to the recruitment of nectar receiver bees.

  • Apis mellifera
  • tremble dance
  • nectar foragers
  • nectar receivers
  • search time
  • unloading delay
  • waggle dance
  • foraging efficiency
  • honey bee

Introduction

Communication plays an important role in the organization of many animal societies. The nectar foraging system of honey bees (Apis mellifera) has received much attention in this context (e.g. von Frisch, 1967; Seeley, 1995), because it provides a convenient system for studying the role of communication in the coordination of colony members. Honey bees use several communication signals to adjust a colony's nectar foraging efficiency, of which the waggle dance (von Frisch, 1967) and the tremble dance (von Frisch, 1923; Seeley, 1992; Seeley et al., 1996) of nectar foragers are especially conspicuous. Lindauer (1948), Schneider (1949), Schick (1953) and von Frisch (1967) reported several factors that seemed to cause tremble dancing. A reliable stimulus and a functional explanation for the tremble dance were, however, reported only recently (Seeley, 1992; Seeley et al., 1996). By keeping all other factors constant, Seeley (1992) showed that, upon return to the hive, a forager performs the tremble dance when she experiences difficulty in finding a receiver bee that unloads her nectar. Thus, a long unloading delay stimulates tremble dancing, which apparently recruits more nectar receiver bees (Seeley et al., 1996). Nectar foragers that experience only short delays before the first unloading contact perform the waggle dance (Seeley, 1992), which recruits additional nectar foragers (von Frisch, 1967). Both dances are performed only when the food source is profitable enough to justify an adjustment of the colony's labor allocation. Thus, both the waggle dance and the tremble dance help to match the work capacities of nectar foragers and nectar receiver bees to avoid inefficient waiting times for either group.

Kirchner and Lindauer (1994) set out to test whether other factors than a long unloading delay can cause tremble dancing, as was suggested by earlier studies (Lindauer, 1948; Schneider, 1949; Schick, 1953; von Frisch, 1967). Unlike Seeley (1992), who kept conditions at the feeder constant during his experiment, these earlier studies stimulated tremble dancing by manipulating the food source, e.g. by forcing foragers to crowd at the feeder (von Frisch, 1967). However, unloading delays were not measured. This would have been important, as a manipulation of the food source may affect the duration of unloading delays (Kirchner, 1993). Therefore, Kirchner and Lindauer (1994) tested whether crowding at the food source caused nectar foragers to tremble dance because it delayed the first unloading contact. They found that crowding stimulated tremble dancing, and that it increased the time to the first unloading contact (initial unloading delay) as well as the entire time before unloading contacts that was not devoted to dancing or unloading (total unloading delay). From this, Kirchner and Lindauer (1994) concluded that crowding caused longer initial, and especially total, unloading delays, which then caused nectar foragers to tremble dance. However, the study did not provide information about how long after their return into the hive nectar foragers started to dance. This information is important, because dancing can occur before unloading, as Kirchner and Lindauer (1994) themselves note. Thus, a forager that starts to tremble dance shortly after her return into the hive can still experience a long delay until her first unloading contact, but this long delay could not have caused the tremble dance. This means that not only the delay until the start of unloading has to be measured, but also the delay until the start of dancing.

I performed experiments to determine whether crowding at the food source caused tremble dancing because it delayed the time to the first unloading contact or the first dance. To do so, I manipulated the density of nectar foragers at the food source by reducing the size of an artificial feeder, and recorded a nectar forager's dance and the time interval between her entrance into the hive and her first unloading contact, or, if she started to dance before she unloaded, to her first dance.

To additionally examine whether tremble dancers from a non-manipulated colony usually experience long unloading delays, I recorded the unloading delays of nectar foragers that returned from natural food sources.

Materials and methods

Study site and observation hives

The study was conducted from May to August, 2001, at the honey bee laboratory of the University of Würzburg, Germany. Observations were made on two colonies (C1 and C2) of the carniolan honey bee Apis mellifera carnica L. The colonies were housed indoors in two-frame observation hives (e.g. von Frisch, 1967), with internal dimensions of 45 cm×45 cm×5 cm and entrance tunnels leading outside. About 18 cm2 of the glass covering the dance floor near the junction of the hive and the entrance tunnel were removed and replaced with a cloth mesh that allowed the marking of bees that exited the hive. To ensure that each bee was observed only once during an experimental phase, each observed bee was marked with a dot of paint on the thorax when she exited the hive. To fix the bee during marking, the cloth mesh was pressed down on the bee when she was moving towards the hive exit. All observations and experiments were made with one colony at a time.

Food source

The food source used for the experiment was a grooved-plate feeder (e.g. Seeley, 1995) that was located 25 m from the hive. The diameter of the feeder varied during the experiments (see `Experiment' below). The feeder provided a concentrated sugar solution [Apiinvert® (Südzucker), 2.4 mol l-1; sugar composition 61% glucose, 39% fructose] and supplied most, or all, of the food collected by the colonies, as natural nectar sources were scarce during this time. Empty feeders were refilled immediately by an assistant.

Experiment

During experiments in July and August 2001, I recorded the time interval between a nectar forager's entrance into the hive and the start of her first unloading contact or first dance, and the type of her dance (waggle dance, tremble dance or no dance) before and during the manipulation of an artificial food source. I recorded data during four experiments with each C1 and C2 for a total of eight experiments. Experiments started between 09:00 and 13:00 h and consisted of a control phase that was followed by a manipulation phase. Each control and manipulation phase lasted approximately 50 min. During the control, the feeder had a circumference of 79 cm (diameter 25 cm) and was big enough to allow simultaneous access to all visiting nectar foragers. During the manipulation, the feeder had a circumference of 16 cm (diameter 5 cm), and was too small to allow simultaneous access. To control the demand for nectar receiver bees in the hive, the number of foragers that visited the feeder was kept constant throughout the experiment (see below).

Measuring dances and unloading delay

To determine the number of nectar foragers that performed waggle dances, tremble dances or no dances during each control and manipulation phase of the experiment, I recorded the first dance that each observed forager performed. Nectar foragers that did not dance were observed during their entire stay in the hive. I recorded unloading delay as the time interval between a forager's appearance in the entrance tunnel of the hive and the start of her first unloading contact (tu), or, if she started to dance before she unloaded, the start of her first dance (td). Only trophallactic contacts of 3 s or longer were considered to be unloading contacts. td is also referred to as `unloading delay', because it is the best estimate for the time interval that informs a forager about the availability of nectar receiver bees, usually the forager's unloading delay, that an observer can have for a forager that starts to dance before her first unloading contact.

Controlling the number of nectar foragers

I trained 200 nectar foragers of the observation colony to the feeder before the experiments started. To recognize nectar foragers of the observation colony, I marked bees entering the hive with one color, and added at the feeder another color (for training and marking technique, see von Frisch, 1967). At all times, an assistant captured with forceps any unmarked bees from the feeder and kept them in a wood cage until the training or experiment was finished. To compensate for loss of foragers between experiments, 10–30 additional foragers from the observation colony were allowed to access the feeder after each experiment. Although the total number of marked foragers might have changed between experiments (e.g. decreased due to death, or increased due to additional marking of foragers), it is unlikely that the number of foragers changed significantly during an experiment. To check whether approximately the same number of foragers visited the feeder per unit time during each control and manipulation phase of the experiment, the feeder assistant recorded every 5th min the number of nectar foragers at the feeder during 3 control and 2 manipulation phases in experiments with C1, and during 4 control and 3 manipulation phases in experiments with C2. The assistant did not record the number of nectar foragers when the tending of the feeder needed undivided attention.

Nectar foragers visiting natural food sources

To determine the dances and unloading delays of foragers that visited natural food sources in May and June, 2001, I observed 63 nectar foragers from C1 on 3 days, and 27 nectar foragers from C2 on 2 days without providing a food source. Observations started at 08:00 h and lasted until 15:00–19:00 h. To reduce the probability of observing bees other than nectar foragers, observations were interrupted when hive bees performed their conspicuous orientation flights. Of the non-dancing bees, only those that had at least one trophallactic contact before they exited the hive again were considered to be nectar foragers. Although I could not distinguish between foragers for water and for nectar, the probability of a forager gathering water instead of nectar is much smaller (e.g. Seeley, 1986). Hence, it is not likely that water foragers introduced a large bias in the study. As foragers were not marked, I was not in all cases (approx. 29%) able to observe non-dancing nectar foragers throughout their entire stay in the hive. As foragers were most likely to waggle dance, the sample size for non-dancing nectar foragers might be biased upward. Bees that were lost out of sight had on average been observed for approx. 75% of the time that full observations lasted. Thus, the results for tremble dancers and, especially, waggle dancers might be biased downward.

Statistical analysis

To analyze whether tremble dancing was caused by a long unloading delay, I compared the unloading delays of waggle dancers and tremble dancers in each control and manipulation phase of the experiment. I did not compare the unloading delays of a group of dancers between phases, because even if unloading delay increases in the manipulation phase, this increase could only be the cause of tremble dancing if waggle dancers did not experience a similar increase in unloading delay.

Measurements are given as means ± one standard deviation (s.d.). Statistical tests used are given in the text. All data were analyzed using the ME edition of Microsoft Excel and the 2002 edition of Statistica. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were performed according to Sokal and Rohlf (1995). The adjustedα -level is noted in the text.

Results

Experiment

I recorded the time to the first unloading contact tu or first dance td, and types of dances (waggle, tremble or no dance) of 210 nectar foragers during the control phase, and of 229 nectar foragers during the manipulation phase.

Number of nectar foragers did not change during experiment

To estimate the number of nectar foragers that simultaneously visited the feeder, an assistant recorded the number of nectar foragers at the feeder every 5th min during 7 control phases and 5 manipulation phases. In C1, the average number of nectar foragers at the feeder was 72±10 foragers min-1 during the control phase (N=3), and 76±11 foragers min-1 during the manipulation phase (N=2). In C2, the average number of nectar foragers was 47±11 foragers min-1 during the control phase (N=4), and 57±6 foragers min-1 during the manipulation phase (N=3). The number of nectar foragers at the feeder was lower during the control phase than during the manipulation phase, but this difference was not significant for either colony (Mann–Whitney U-test; P>0.275 for each comparison). For the following analysis, data from C1 and C2 were pooled.

Manipulation increased tremble dancing rate

During the control phase, 36 nectar foragers performed a tremble dance, 115 performed a waggle dance, and 59 did not dance. During the manipulation phase, 161 nectar foragers performed a tremble dance, 23 performed a waggle dance, and 45 did not dance. The probability for a nectar forager to tremble dance was significantly higher during the manipulation phase than during the control phase of the experiment (Fig. 1, Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.005, N=8). The probability to waggle dance was significantly lower during the manipulation phase than during the control phase (P<0.001), and the probability to not dance did not change (P=0.431).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

The probability that a nectar forager performs a waggle dance, a tremble dance or no dance during each control phase and manipulation phase of the experiment. Statistics are given in the text. Data were pooled for Colonies 1 and 2.

Most nectar foragers danced before they unloaded

Fig. 2 shows the percentage of nectar foragers that started to dance before they unloaded. During the control phase of the experiment, 57% of the waggle dancers (N=115) and 92% of the tremble dancers (N=36) started to dance before they had their first unloading contact. During the manipulation phase of the experiment, 48% of the waggle dancers (N=23) and 90% of the tremble dancers (N=161) started to dance before they had their first unloading contact.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

The percentage of waggle dancers, tremble dancers and non-dancing nectar foragers that had an unloading contact before they started to dance (white bars), and that started to dance before they had an unloading contact (black bars). (A) Data for the control phase, and (B) for the manipulation phase of the experiment. Statistics are given in the test. Data were pooled for Colonies 1 and 2.

Tremble dancers and waggle dancers did not differ in tuor td

Fig. 3 shows dancing (waggle dancing, tremble dancing or no dancing) as a function of the time interval between a forager's entrance into the hive and her first unloading contact (tu) or first dance (td). Mean values and sample sizes are given in Table 1. Tremble dancers and waggle dancers did not differ in tu during either the control phase (Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.666) or the manipulation phase of the experiment (Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.352). Tremble dancers and waggle dancers did not differ in td during the control phase (Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.784), but during the manipulation phase, tremble dancers had a significantly shorter td than waggle dancers (Mann–Whitney U-test; P<0.001). Non-dancing nectar foragers did not differ in tu from waggle dancers or tremble dancers (Mann–Whitney U-test, P>0.07 for all comparisons).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

The distribution of unloading delays for waggle dancers, tremble dancers and non-dancing nectar foragers that either unloaded first (tu; A,C) or danced first (td; B,D). (A) and (B) show the data for the control phase, (C) and (D) for the manipulation phase of the experiment. Sample sizes are given in Table 1. Note that only three tremble dancers unloaded first during the control phase of the experiment. Data from Colonies 1 and 2 are pooled. Statistics are given in the text.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Measurements of unloading delay before contact (tu) or dance (td) for nectar foragers during the control and manipulation phases of the experiment and for nectar foragers that returned from natural food sources

Nectar foragers visiting natural food sources

To record unloading delays of unmanipulated nectar foragers, I observed 38 waggle dancers, 23 tremble dancers, and 29 non-dancing nectar foragers that had visited natural food sources. 68% of the waggle dancers and 78% of the tremble dancers danced before they had their first unloading contact. Tremble dancers and waggle dancers did not differ in either tu (Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.527) or td (Mann–Whitney U-test; P=0.830). Non-dancing nectar foragers had a significantly longer tu than either tremble dancers or waggle dancers (Mann–Whitney U-test; adjusted α-level 0.017, P>0.020). Values are given in Table 1.

Discussion

This study shows that tremble dancing can be stimulated by factors other than a long unloading delay. In particular, crowding of nectar foragers at the food source caused a fourfold increase in tremble dancing, which was not due to long delays to either the first unloading contact or the first dance. It is unlikely that tremble dancers reacted to long unloading delays after unobserved earlier trips, because nectar foragers experienced generally short unloading delays before they started to tremble dance.

Most tremble dancers (approximately 91%) started to dance before they had their first unloading contact, and usually started to dance as early as, or earlier than, waggle dancers. Because tremble dancers may dance for up to 1.5 h, and rarely stop dancing to unload (Seeley, 1992; Thom et al., 2003), these tremble dancers could have had their first unloading contact late during their stay in the hive. Therefore, Kirchner and Lindauer (1994), who measured the time to the first unloading contact, but not to the first dance, are likely to have reported longer unloading delays than tremble dancers actually experienced.

In comparison to the study by Seeley (1992), I observed (1) a higher level of tremble dancing during both control and manipulation phases of the experiment, and (2) possibly more tremble dancers that danced before they had the first unloading contact. Both observations suggest that nectar foragers had a higher motivation to tremble dance in this study than in Seeley's 1992 study. During the manipulation phase of the experiment, a higher motivation for tremble dancing could have been the different tremble dance stimulus (crowding instead of long unloading delays). During the control phase, nectar foragers may have had a higher motivation to tremble dance, e.g. because unwanted foragers may have released alarm pheromone when they were captured, or the many nectar foragers that visited the feeder simultaneously (47–76 bees) interfered at a low level with each other. Kirchner and Lindauer (1994), who also trained 200 foragers to a well-sized feeder, found even higher levels of tremble dancing (32% of all foragers) than this study (17% of all foragers). In general, crowding may have affected several parameters at the food source, including access to the feeder, efficiency of food loading, or concentration of alarm pheromone, which could have motivated nectar foragers to tremble dance.

Although the exact parameters that crowding changes at a food source are not yet identified, the short unloading delays of tremble dancers are not likely to be artefacts. Many tremble dancers that had visited natural food sources had equal, or shorter, unloading delays than waggle dancers. This suggests that tremble dancing is an adaptive reaction to stimuli external to the hive. These stimuli, like crowding, are likely to be related to a decrease in foraging efficiency. One example is given by von Frisch (1967), who reports that tremble dancing can be elicited by sticky food sources such as Asclepias (Asclepiadaceae) flowers. Therefore, the results of this study suggest that the tremble dance may have an additional function to the recruitment of nectar receiver bees. Tremble dancing in this study was not caused by a situation usually associated with a shortage of nectar receiver bees, and hence may not serve to adjust the number of nectar receiver bees. Instead, when tremble dancing is a reaction to a decrease in foraging efficiency at the food source, it may be supposed to direct foragers away from this, and possibly toward other, food sources.

It may be important to notice that not only tremble dancers, but also waggle dancers often (48–57%) started to dance before they had their first unloading contact. Hence, these waggle dancers did not seem to have information about the availability of nectar receiver bees but, like tremble dancers, may have been stimulated to dance mostly by the quality of the food source. As neither the waggle dance nor the tremble dance seem to always regulate the number of nectar receiver bees, it can be supposed that nectar receiver bees regulate their activity themselves. For example, nectar receiver bees may often adjust their activity by assessing and reacting to the conditions on the dance floor. Unlike waiting, and thus inactive, foragers who miss valuable opportunities to gather nectar, inactive nectar receiver bees may impose a relatively small, or no, net cost on the colony. This could be because nectar receiver bees are usually middle-aged workers (Seeley et al., 1996), which have been suggested to be often unemployed (Seeley, 1995; Kühnholz and Seeley, 1997). Furthermore, nectar receiver bees might enhance colony foraging efficiency not only by unloading nectar foragers, but also by sampling the quality and availability of nectar, which may later enable them to chose more profitable food sources. Hence, it can be speculated that the net cost to the colony that these unemployed workers impose by waiting for nectar foragers may be low, and often negligible. Therefore, nectar receiver bees may often regulate their activity based on information they acquired by sampling nectar foragers, and only be recruited by the tremble dance when a sudden nectar influx requires an exceptionally fast increase in the number of nectar receiver bees.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I thank David C. Gilley, Prof. Harald E. Esch, Dr David R. Tarpy and Prof. Jürgen Tautz for reading and discussing the manuscript, Elizabeth A. Tibbetts for discussions, and two anonymous referees for valuable comments. I thank Susanne Reuther, Holger Steger and Heinrich Demel for help with bees and hives. This study was financed by the GK 200 and the SFB 554.

FOOTNOTES

  • Present address: ARL Division of Neurobiology, Gould-Simpson Building, PO Box 21077, Tucson, AZ 85721-0077, USA (e-mail: thom{at}neurobio.arizona.edu)

  • © The Company of Biologists Limited 2003

References

  1. ↵
    Kirchner, W. H. (1993). Vibrational signals in the tremble dance of the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33,169 -172.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    Kirchner, W. H. and Lindauer, M. (1994). The causes of the tremble dance. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35,303 -308.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    Kühnholz, S. and Seeley, T. D. (1997). The control of water collection in honey bee colonies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41,407 -422.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    Lindauer, M. (1948). Über die Einwirkung von Duftund Geschmackstoffen sowie anderer Faktoren auf die Tänze der Bienen. Z. vergl. Physiol. 31,348 -412.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    Schick, W. (1953). Über die Wirkung von Giftstoffen auf die Tänze der Bienen. Z. vergl. Physiol. 35,105 -128.
    OpenUrl
  6. ↵
    Schneider, F. (1949). Über die Vergiftung der Bienen mit Dinitrokresol. Mitt. Schweiz. Entomol. Gesellsch. 33,223 -237.
    OpenUrl
  7. ↵
    Seeley, T. D. (1986). Social foraging by honey bees: how colonies allocate foragers among patches of flowers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19,343 -354.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  8. ↵
    Seeley, T. D. (1992). The tremble dance of the honey bee: message and meanings. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 31,375 -383.
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    Seeley, T. D. (1995). The Wisdom of the Hive. Boston: Harvard University Press.
  10. ↵
    Seeley, T. D., Kühnholz, S. and Weidenmüller, A. (1996). The honey bee's tremble dance stimulates additional bees to function as nectar receiver bees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 39,419 -427.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    Sokal, R. R. and Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry. 3rd edition. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.
  12. ↵
    Thom, C., Gilley, D. C. and Tautz, J. (2003). Worker piping in honey bees: the behavior of piping nectar foragers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53,199 -205.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    von Frisch, K. (1923). Über die Sprache der Bienen, eine tierpsychologische Untersuchung. Zool. JB. (Physiol.) 40,1 -186.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    von Frisch, K. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The tremble dance of honey bees can be caused by hive-external foraging experience
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
The tremble dance of honey bees can be caused by hive-external foraging experience
Corinna Thom
Journal of Experimental Biology 2003 206: 2111-2116; doi: 10.1242/jeb.00398
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
The tremble dance of honey bees can be caused by hive-external foraging experience
Corinna Thom
Journal of Experimental Biology 2003 206: 2111-2116; doi: 10.1242/jeb.00398

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • Introduction
    • Materials and methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • FOOTNOTES
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Sex-specific microhabitat use is associated with sex-biased thermal physiology in Anolis lizards
  • Early developmental stages of native populations of Ciona intestinalis under increased temperature are affected by local habitat history
  • Three auditory brainstem response (ABR) methods tested and compared in two anuran species
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLE

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Meet the Editors at SICB Virtual 2021

Reserve your place to join some of the journal editors, including Editor-in-Chief Craig Franklin, at our Meet the Editor session on 17 February at 2pm (EST). Don’t forget to view our SICB Subject Collection, featuring relevant JEB papers relating to some of the symposia sessions.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020's challenges, we were able to bring a number of long-term projects and new ventures to fruition. As we enter a new year, join us as we reflect on the triumphs of the last 12 months.


The Big Biology podcast

JEB author Christine Cooper talks to Big Biology about her research. In this fascinating JEB sponsored podcast she tells us how tough zebra finches adjust their physiology to cope with extreme climate events. 


Developmental and reproductive physiology of small mammals at high altitude

Cayleih Robertson and Kathryn Wilsterman focus on high-altitude populations of the North American deer mouse in their review of the challenges and evolutionary innovations of pregnant and nursing small mammals at high altitude.


Read & Publish participation extends worldwide

“Being able to publish Open Access articles free of charge means that my article gets maximum exposure and has maximum impact, and that all my peers can read it regardless of the agreements that their universities have with publishers.”

Professor Roi Holzman (Tel Aviv University) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 60 institutions in 12 countries taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992