Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Experimental Biology
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Journal of Experimental Biology

  • Log in
Advanced search

RSS  Twitter  Facebook  YouTube  

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Special issues
    • Subject collections
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About JEB
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Travelling Fellowships
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
    • Outstanding paper prize
    • Biology Open transfer
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contacts
    • Contact JEB
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Research Article
Oxygen uptake during post dive recovery in a diving bird Aythya fuligula: implications for optimal foraging models
Roland Parkes, Lewis G. Halsey, Anthony J. Woakes, Roger L. Holder, Patrick J. Butler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2002 205: 3945-3954;
Roland Parkes
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lewis G. Halsey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anthony J. Woakes
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Roger L. Holder
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Patrick J. Butler
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF
Loading

SUMMARY

The rate of oxygen uptake at the surface between dives was measured for four tufted ducks, Aythya fuligula, during bouts of foraging dives to a depth of 1.8 m. The ducks surfaced into a respirometer box after each dive so that the rate of oxygen uptake (V̇O2) could be measured. V̇O2 decreased over time at the surface and there was a particularly rapid phase of oxygen uptake for approximately the first 3s. The specific shape of the oxygen uptake curve is dependent upon the duration of the preceding dive. The uptake curve after longer dives was significantly steeper during the first 3s at the surface than after shorter dives, although V̇O2 after the first 3s was not significantly different between these two dive duration bins. Thus, the mean total oxygen uptake (VO2) was higher after surface periods following longer dives. Due to the high V̇O2 during the initial part of the surface period, the curve associated with longer dives was statistically biphasic, with the point of inflection at 3.3s. The curve for shorter dives was not statistically biphasic. The birds may increase their respiratory frequency during the first 3s after longer dives, producing the increased V̇O2, which would enable the birds to resaturate their oxygen stores more rapidly in response to the increased oxygen depletion of the longer submergence time.

  • tufted duck
  • Aythya fuligula
  • diving
  • oxygen uptake
  • optimal foraging
  • model

Introduction

`Central place forager' is a term used to describe the behaviour of a foraging animal that repeatedly returns to the same place with food (Orians and Pearson, 1979; Lessells and Stephens, 1983). The close analogy between a bird returning to its nest with food (Kacelnik, 1984) and a diving animal returning to the surface to breathe means that the concept of central place foraging can be applied to surfacing divers (Houston and McNamara, 1985). In this case, the surface of the water is the `central place'. Thus, models that examine the behaviour of central place foragers can be transposed to examine the behaviour of a diving animal.

During foraging bouts, assuming that a diver wants to be under the water acquiring food for as much time as possible, repeatedly returning to the surface for oxygen is (generally) in direct competition with food gain (Ydenberg and Clark, 1989). Optimal foraging models, which are based on the marginal value theorem (Charnov, 1976), have been developed to determine the optimal surface time for oxygen loading that maximises the proportion of time spent foraging (Kramer, 1988; Houston and Carbone, 1992; Carbone and Houston, 1996). These models are based on the physiological gains and losses of oxygen over the dive cycle and therefore the postdive loading curve is an integral, but as yet unquantified, part of many theoretical studies of optimal time allocation (Thompson et al., 1993; Carbone and Houston, 1994, 1996; Lea et al., 1996; Walton et al., 1998; Mori, 1998, 1999).

Because the mammalian lung system rapidly collapses on immersion (Kooyman and Ponganis, 1998), the majority of the oxygen stores of marine mammals consist of haemoglobin and myoglobin. Thus it is likely that the generic shape for the oxygen loading curve proposed and used by Kramer (1988) in his optimal breathing model (Fig. 1) may be correct for most diving mammals. However, Walton et al. (1998) have argued that this shape is not accurate for diving birds.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

The optimal breathing model (Kramer, 1988). The abscissa shows time spent travelling to and from the foraging site to the left of the ordinate, and time at the surface to the right. The ordinate shows the amount of oxygen consumed during travel and gained during surface periods. The value ts* denotes the optimal surface time for the diver, according to the model, in terms of maximising the proportion of time at the foraging site.

The respiratory system of birds differs from that of mammals and consists mainly of large distended air sacs and a lung, which is comparatively small and rigid (Scheid, 1979). However, the estimated contribution of the air sacs to the total body oxygen stores of diving birds ranges from 23% to 64%, e.g. tufted duck (Keijer and Butler, 1982), thick-billed murre (Croll et al., 1992), lesser scaup (Stephenson, 1995), Adélie penguin (Kooyman and Ponganis, 1998), king penguin (Ponganis et al., 1999). Therefore, it has been suggested that the respiratory system will have a profound influence on the dynamics of post-dive oxygen loading. The oxygen stored within the air sacs of tufted ducks during a dive is made available for consumption through locomotion, and associated abdominal activity. This produces pressure differentials between the posterior and anterior air sacs, which could reciprocally force the gas stored in the air sacs through the lungs (Boggs et al., 1998). Walton et al. (1998) suggest that the gas in the air sacs at the end of a dive is therefore depleted of oxygen. They argued that, at the start of a surface interval, fresh air must be taken up into the respiratory tract before oxygen uptake by haemoglobin and myoglobin can occur, since fresh air must enter the caudal air sacs before any oxygen becomes available to the lungs.

Consequently, Walton et al. (1998) predict that avian divers will produce a `kinked' oxygen uptake curve, with the first phase representing oxygen taken more rapidly into the air sacs and the second representing the relatively slower replacement of haemoglobin and myoglobin stores. In contrast to the smooth curve, this biphasic modification of the oxygen resaturation curve results in the prediction that all dives shorter than a certain duration will have identical optimal surface times (Walton et al., 1998) (Fig. 2). In addition, these dives will also achieve identical levels of oxygen resaturation during the subsequent surface period (Walton et al., 1998).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Model, as in Fig. 1, incorporating a biphasic oxygen uptake curve (Walton et al., 1998). Due to the initial rapid resaturation of oxygen into the respiratory oxygen stores upon surfacing, a biphasic oxygen resaturation curve means that a range of dive depths (d1-d3), associated with travel time (tT), have identical optimal surface times (ts*).

Both the smooth oxygen uptake curves of earlier models as well as the biphasic curve of Walton et al. (1998) predict a number of optimal behavioural patterns. These concern adjustments to surface time and foraging time in response to changes in dive depth and energetic costs during the dive. Some qualitative behavioural trends recorded in diving birds are predicted by optimal time allocation models incorporating the smooth oxygen replacement curve (Carbone and Houston, 1994; Carbone et al., 1996; Lea et al., 1996), while trends reported by Walton et al. (1998) are predicted by their model, which incorporates the biphasic curve. However, it is premature to affirm the success of any of these models when key assumptions have not been verified. It is likely that the details of oxygen uptake curves have a critical effect on the gross predictions of optimal foraging models for divers (Ruxton et al., 2000). The differing predictions of present models reveal the importance of empirical study on oxygen uptake curves so that further progress can be made in understanding observed diving behaviour.

The purpose of the present study was to test the hypothesis that the post-dive oxygen gain curve for tufted ducks Aythya fuligula is biphasic. The delineation of the loading curve for voluntarily diving birds was achieved using a fast-response respirometry system, which is able to measure small changes in oxygen concentration over a fairly fine time resolution given a repetitive measurement signal. This enabled the quantification of a critical aspect of optimal foraging models for diving vertebrates and the determination of the extent to which the biphasic adaptation for diving birds is apparent.

Materials and methods

Four tufted ducks, Aythya fuligula L., were used for the experiment, two males and two females, of mean body mass 643±26 g. The birds were raised from eggs and housed together in a large indoor tank [1.6×1×1.7 m (maximum depth)] at Birmingham University. The ducks were fed corn and growers' pellets (Dodson and Hornell Ltd, Kettering, UK) from a dry area (1.0×0.5 m) mounted on the side of the tank. White grit and small amounts of corn were thrown onto the water daily to encourage natural diving behaviour.

The experimental dive tank was located in a large outdoor aviary close to where the animals were housed. Two large metal fencing panels (1.75×3.45 m) were bolted together perpendicularly and placed in one corner of the dive tank, creating a 1.75×1.75 m cross-section area with a maximum depth of 2.7 m, in which the experiments were conducted. The metal panels stood 0.75 m above the water and, by using smaller fencing panels as a roof and additional side netting, a corral was formed above the water to prevent the birds from escaping.

The birds were trained to dive to a feeding platform (67×82 cm) positioned at a depth of 1.8 m. During the experiments the birds were fed corn and live mealworms. The latter was found to encourage diving behaviour. A false mesh floor was fitted to the bottom of the tank to prevent birds from diving for food that had spilt from the feeding platform. A submersible pool cleaner (Dolphin, Maytronics Ltd) was periodically used to clean fallen food from the floor of the tank. The water temperature varied between 10 and 15°C during the experimental period.

Respirometry experiments

The surface of the tank was covered in netting except for a small area over which the open base of the small, clear acrylic respirometer chamber (35×25×25 cm) was positioned and from which the individual birds dived. The open base of the respirometer was placed 4 cm below the surface of the water to form an airtight seal along its sides. This made the effective volume of the respirometer 18 375 ml. The respirometer contained a fan that ensured thorough mixing of the air within the chamber. A flow of 500 ml s-1 was drawn through the respirometer by a fixed flow pump, keeping CO2 below 0.2% at all times. Metabolic gas concentrations were determined using a zirconia oxygen sensor (Ametek, Thermox Instruments, model S3A-1/N.22) and an infrared carbon dioxide sensor (ADC Ltd, model SS-100) connected in series. The sample pump in the CO2 sensor was used to draw an air sample from an outlet in the respirometer and through the gas sensors at a constant flow rate of 17 ml s-1. A 500 ml flask was used just prior to the sample pump to reduce any flow oscillations (see Fig. 3 for a diagram of the experimental apparatus). Total flow through the respirometer was 517 ml s-1. Tubing of 300 ml volume was attached to the holes in the respirometer open to ambient air. This ensured that when the duck initially surfaced into the respirometer after a dive, the small volume of air forced out of the holes did not escape from the system and was subsequently sucked back into the respirometer.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Diagram of the experimental apparatus showing a tufted duck in an enclosed section of a large dive tank. For further details, see Materials and methods.

All connecting tubing was impermeable to oxygen and was as short and as small bore as possible to reduce dead space. The response time of the oxygen analyser was less than 0.2 s and the lag time of the respirometer box and tubing was 2.5 s. The residual time constant of the system after deconvolution (see later) was 0.4 s and was determined by N2 injections at various points within the respirometer to simulate instantaneous changes in oxygen uptake. The effect of the residual time constant is a slight filtering of the VO2 data, which results in a slight blurring of the cumulative oxygen uptake curves.

The O2 and CO2 analysers were calibrated each day with known gas concentrations produced by a precision gas-mixing pump (Wösthoff Pumps, Bochum). Atmospheric pressure was recorded every hour (Digital Weather Barometer, Prosser Scientific Instruments Ltd, UK) and a relative humidity and temperature probe (Vaisala, Finland) was fixed inside the respirometer to allow airflow through the respirometer to be corrected for water vapour.

A LabVIEW program (National Instruments Corporation, USA) was written to record data on respirometer oxygen concentration during the experimental sessions. A subroutine of the program controlled a reference valve, which sampled ambient oxygen concentration for 10 s every 20 min. This allowed the detection and correction of baseline drift in the measuring system. 300 scans were averaged every 250 ms for humidity, temperature, %O2, %CO2 and reference valve position, and the data were then recorded onto the internal hard drive of the computer running the program (Dell Computers Dimensions XPS P60).

During the experimental sessions, the bird's behaviour was recorded onto video tape to determine time budget data by using a black and white video camera (JVC, model TK-S240) and video recorder (Mitsubishi, model Time Lapse HS-5424E[B]A). An additional small hole in the surface net was replaced with a clear perspex sheet, through which activity at the feeding platform could also be filmed. Two 500 W lamps were attached to the roof of the corral and used to illuminate the feeding platform. A light-emitting diode placed in the field of view of the video camera was turned on at the start of each data recording session. This allowed accurate synchrony between the video footage of behaviour and the respirometer oxygen composition data so that rates of oxygen uptake against time for each surface period could be determined. Preliminary data showed that the ducks first inspired when they were fully surfaced on the water.

Analysis of respirometry data

Percentage oxygen concentration data were converted into rate of oxygen uptake using a modification of the formula given by Woakes and Butler (1983), which allows accurate determination of fast changes in gas concentrations (see Equation 1 below). By treating the respirometer as both an open and a closed system, oxygen uptake could be determined for 0.25 s intervals, despite the system having a much longer response time (36 s). The changes in oxygen concentration in the respirometer between t1 and t2 were very small and often within the error of the oxygen analyser, creating a low ratio of measurement signal against signal noise. However, the noise component of each measurement was random and therefore equally likely to be negative or positive. Thus, recording multiple data points for each value of t allowed signal averaging to recover the measurement signal at each 0.25 s by reducing the magnitude of the mean noise value (Bentley, 1983), i.e. averaging maintained the root-mean-square (rms) value of the measurement signal while reducing the rms value of the noise. Because a very large number of data points (N=870 and 878) were collected and averaged, the signal-to-noise ratio was greatly increased. A change of 1 ml O2 in the respirometer could then be measured with an error of approximately± 0.03 ml: Math1 where VO2 = total oxygen consumption between times t1 and t2 (ml), Ox1 and Ox2 = fractional concentrations of oxygen at times t1, t2 leaving the chamber, V = respirometer volume (ml), Oxamb = fractional concentration of (ambient) oxygen entering the respirometer, t1 and t2 = start and finish, respectively, of a period of time where variation of oxygen concentration in the respirometer is recorded, and Q̇ = flow rate out of the respirometer (ml s-1). All gas concentrations and volumes were corrected to STPD.

Oxygen data for the last 1 s surface interval were removed so that any elevated pre-dive V̇O2 was not included in the analysis (Butler and Woakes, 1979; Woakes and Butler, 1983). For analysis of oxygen uptake data, values are means ± S.E.M. for four animals. To avoid animal bias, mean values were obtained for each bird and these means were used to obtain the final mean. A significant difference between means was tested with paired t-tests.

Statistics for biphasic determination

Bout criterion interval analysis (Slater and Lester, 1982) was used to eliminate all (inter-bout) dives with a surface time greater than 35 s. Preliminary data suggested that longer dives had a more pronounced effect on the oxygen loading dynamics, possibly due to the amount of oxygen used while submerged, and so the dive data were split into two groups for analysis. Dives were split at 16.0 s, which gave two dive duration bins, `short' and `long', with N-values as equal as possible (short, dive duration<16.0 s, N=870; long, dive duration ≥16.0 s, N=878). The mean VO2 data were smoothed by taking a running three-point average and then log-transformed to linearise the exponentially decreasing oxygen uptake data. `Broken stick' analysis was then performed to see if the biphasic regression line was statistically a better fit than a linear one (Seber, 1977). To construct a biphasic regression line, the logged mean oxygen uptake data were split at a selected data point. A linear regression line was then fitted to the data, which corresponded from the start of the surface time up to and including the chosen break point. Similarly, another linear regression line was fitted just after the chosen break point to the remainder of the data. The uptake data during the surface recovery had therefore been split into two phases around a chosen data point. The linear equations for each of these phases allowed the intersecting point of inflection, C, to be calculated (see Equation 5) and an overall biphasic regression equation to be determined (see Equations 6, 7).

An illustration of this technique can be seen in Fig. 4. For this worked example the break point in the data was arbitrarily chosen at 6 s. Regression analysis was conducted on the data up to and including this break point (Linear phase 1) and again on all remaining data points (Linear phase 2). The point of intersection for these two regression lines also represents the point of inflection, C, of the biphasic regression line. Equation 5 can therefore be used with parameters determined by the linear regression analysis to calculate this point of inflection. A generalised regression equation for a biphasic line can then be produced using Equations 6 and 7. It is at this point that the `broken stick' analysis is conducted to see if the biphasic regression line is statistically a better description of the data than a linear regression line (Fig. 4B).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Charts to show the methodology of biphasic modelling using broken stick analysis. (A) VO2 data for four tufted ducks were smoothed and plotted on a logarithmic axis. The break point was arbitrarily chosen at 6 s. Linear regression was performed up to and including the break point (Linear phase 1) and also on all remaining data points (Linear phase 2). Regression parameters were then used to determine the point of inflection (C) where the two lines intersect. (B) VO2 data for four tufted ducks were regressed onto the two regression lines shown (biphasic and linear). The biphasic regression line was calculated as shown in Fig. 4A. Commonly in this type of analysis the biphasic point of inflection (C) was not identical to the selected break point.

Locating inflection points for biphasic regression

Least-squares regression was performed on the uptake data with the break point at 6 s post dive, as given in the example (Fig. 4). An iterative sequence of least-squares regression with the chosen break point decreasing by 0.25 s every execution was then completed. The biphasic regression line that had a break point with the lowest residual variance, and so represented the best description of the data, was used as the biphasic regression solution to the uptake data.

For the biphasic regression, both phases of the broken stick model were calculated by linear regression and so can be defined as follows:

Linear phase 1 (before the break): Math2 and Linear phase 2 (after the break): Math3 The point of intersection for Equations 2 and 3 can be determined with respect to x by solving Equations 1 and 2: Math4 Math5 Since this point of intersection also represents the point of inflection for biphasic oxygen uptake, C, the generalised regression equations for a biphasic line, where a1, a2 and b1 are parameters for the linear components of the broken stick model are: Math6 Math7

To test the significance of the biphasic regression equation, least-squares regression was performed using both the biphasic construct and simple linear regression. An F-test was then performed on the residual mean sum of squares for both equations to determine statistically whether the biphasic regression was a significant improvement. Degrees of freedom (d.f.) for linear regression is N-1 whereas d.f. for biphasic uptake is N-4 as a1, a2, b1 and C have been calculated to define the biphasic equation. Other methodologies are available to determine whether biphasic relationships exist within the data. These are more complex as they allow for a smoother transition between the two phases of the biphasic construct (Koops and Grossman, 1993).

Results

Diving time budgets

The data for four tufted ducks were split into bins of short duration dives (<16.0 s; N=870) and long duration dives (>16.0 s; N=878). The time budget data were therefore not normally distributed within the groups. Table 1 contains a summary of these data. The oxygen uptake data for individual birds in both groups were normally distributed about the mean (Kolmogorov—Smirnov test; not significant).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Summary of time budget data

Oxygen uptake

The mean rate of oxygen uptake (V̇O2) for all dives, over the first 15 s, was 1.14±0.33 ml s-1. However, the rate of oxygen uptake was not constant (Fig. 5). The VO2 data for all dives show that upon surfacing there was a more rapid phase of oxygen uptake for approximately the first 3 s, during which V̇O2 was 2.45±0.09 ml s-1.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

Bar chart showing mean oxygen uptake for four tufted ducks during the first 14.5 s post surface (values are means + S.E.M.). The line graphs show the cumulative oxygen gain curve for short (<16.0 s; solid line) and long dives (≥16.0 s; broken line) for the first 14.5 s post dive.

V̇O2 was significantly higher for the first 3 s of surface time after both short and long dives (short dives, 2.22±0.13 ml s-1; long dives, 2.67±0.12 ml s-1) than for the rest of the surface period, up to 15 s (short dives, 0.72±0.28 ml s-1, P<0.001; long dives, 0.82±0.37 ml s-1, P<0.001). V̇O2 significantly differed between short and long dives during the first 3 s (P<0.01) but did not significantly differ between short and long dives for the rest of the surface period up to 15 s.

After long dives, the mean total oxygen uptake during the first 15 s of the surface interval (17.9 ml) was significantly higher than that after short dives (15.3 ml, P<0.001; Fig. 6). Statistical comparison of the mean uptake curves for the two dive duration bins was achieved by testing for a significant difference between the cumulative oxygen values of the curves at 5 s, 10 s and 15 s. The values at 5 s, 10 s and 15 s were significantly higher in the oxygen uptake curve associated with longer duration dives than in the shorter ones (Table 2).

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Comparison of the mean oxygen uptake curve of four tufted ducks at 5 s, 10 s and 15 s surface duration ± S.E.M. after short dives and long dives.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Statistical comparison between oxygen uptake curves after dives of short and long duration, and for all dives combined

Biphasic analysis

The break points with the lowest residual variance used to calculate the biphasic regression lines were 5.00 s for short dives and 3.00 s for long dives (Fig. 7). Using these values in Equations 1, 2 and 5 gave the point of inflection for biphasic oxygen uptake, C, as 3.3 s for short dives and 3.3 s for long dives. Least-squares regression analysis was performed using both linear and biphasic equations on log transformed smoothed mean VO2 data for both long (Fig. 7) and short dives.

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

Histogram showing the calculated residual variances with the break point every 0.25 s for the first 6 s post dive for long dives of four tufted ducks. The break point with the lowest residual variance occurred 3.00 s post dive and was used to define the biphasic regression line. See statistics in Materials and methods for details of the biphasic determination.

All linear and biphasic regression lines used were highly significant (Table 3). F-tests were then used on the residual mean sum of squares for the linear and the biphasic regression lines to see whether the biphasic constructs were a significantly better description of the data. Biphasic regression was not significantly better for short dives (C=3.34, F56,53=1.45, not significant). For long dives, the biphasic regression was a significantly better fit for the data than linear regression (C=3.28, F56.53=1.96, P<0.01). Thus, oxygen uptake is only biphasic for long duration dives, with the point of inflection at 3.3 s (Fig. 8). Due to the effect of the residual time constant on the cumulative oxygen uptake curves, the points of inflection may be slightly inaccurate. Nevertheless, the true point of inflection for both these curves is very close to 3.3 s.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Results of biphasic determination

Fig. 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 8.

Chart to show biphasic modelling for long duration dives during the first 15 s post surface for four tufted ducks. The smoothed VO2 data (open circles) have been log-transformed. Least-squares regression is then performed using the linear and the biphasic regression lines. Phases 1 and 2 of the biphasic regression line can be seen with the point of inflection at 3.28 s post surface.

Discussion

The dive durations, surface durations and oxygen uptake volumes at the surface in the present study are comparable to time budget data recorded from previous studies on the same genus diving to similar depths (Table 4).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Mean values of time budget data for similar respirometer studies with Aythya species diving to similar depths

The rate of oxygen uptake V̇O2 changes over time at the surface after both long and short dives. In both cases, V̇O2 is higher at the start of the surface period and decreases against time. This relationship is not linear, as shown by the oxygen uptake curves for long and short dives in Fig. 5. Rather, both curves can be seen to consist of far higher V̇O2 for approximately the first 3 s of surface time.

The shape of the oxygen uptake curve changes with the duration of the dive (Figs 5, 6) and oxygen was taken up significantly faster during surface periods associated with longer dive durations. This suggests that, as dive duration increases, and presumably total oxygen consumed during the dive (VO2d) increases, initial surface V̇O2 increases. While V̇O2 from 3 s to 15 s was not significantly different between the two conditions, it was significantly higher after longer dives during the first 3 s. Whether this is due partly to an increase in effort by the birds to take up oxygen more quickly, or is purely a passive phenomenon dependent upon the difference in partial pressure of oxygen inside and outside the cardio-respiratory system, is not clear. Higher V̇O2 for the first 3 s after longer dives, coupled with a V̇O2 beyond 3 s that does not vary with dive duration, creates a sharper `kink' in the oxygen uptake curve after longer dives.

Implications for optimal foraging models

The kink in the oxygen uptake curve produced by the ducks for long dive durations was sufficiently pronounced to make the measured curve statistically biphasic, while the uptake curve for short dive durations was not. This indicates that the break in the curve becomes more prominent during surface periods when the dive duration was longer and VO2 was higher. So, for tufted ducks, dives of a relatively short duration are followed by surface periods where rates of oxygen uptake produce a curve similar to that predicted by Kramer (1988). Longer dive durations produce oxygen uptake curves with a clearer biphasic element and so become more like the curve predicted by Walton et al. (1998).

According to the predictions of Walton et al. (1998), a range of dives up to a certain duration will be associated with very similar surface durations because the tangent from travel time tT will be `locked' to the kink of the curve. However, the mean surface duration for long duration dives is more than fourfold greater than the surface duration at which the kink often occurs (15.4 s versus approx. 3.3 s). For tufted ducks, therefore, the volume of oxygen consumed during the dive may be high enough that the tangent routinely touches the curve beyond the kink (Houston, 2000). Shorter dives, where less oxygen is consumed, produce smoother oxygen uptake curves and so there is not such a pronounced kink to intercept the tangent. Extended dives by tufted ducks, although utilising very high volumes of oxygen, might possibly produce oxygen uptake curves with such an acute kink that the tangent still touches it. However, although occasional dives of over 40 s have been reported in tufted ducks (Stephenson et al., 1986), Table 4 indicates that the vast majority of dives by this species are unlikely to be long enough to cause such a large change to the post-dive oxygen reloading dynamics. Furthermore, field studies suggest that dives are normally fairly short, for example Magnusdottir and Einarsson (1990) recorded a mean dive time for tufted ducks diving on Lake Mývatn of 18.3 s.

Respiratory frequency during periods of oxygen uptake

Walton et al. (1998) may have made an incorrect assumption that oxygen must be taken up into the caudal air sacs before it is then made available for gaseous exchange. In fact, on inspiration, a significant proportion of air directly enters the paleopulmonic parabronchi via the main bronchus (Bretz and Schmidt-Nielsen, 1971; Butler et al., 1988; Powell, 2000). Indeed, Powell (2000) suggests that when the tidal volume is large, some inspired gases reach as far as the cranial air sacs, via the paleopulmonic parabronchi, during the same breath. In this case, birds would not exhibit a biphasic oxygen uptake curve for the reasons stated by Walton et al. (1998), since fresh air would reach the lungs on the first inspiration. Nevertheless, tufted ducks do demonstrate biphasic oxygen uptake after relatively long dives and so there must be an alternative explanation for this kinked curve.

V̇O2 not only decreases against surface duration but shows a cyclical aspect that is not attenuated when the data are smoothed after long duration dives (Fig. 9). This cyclical aspect is not as clear after short duration dives. After long duration dives, the peaks in VO2 data reflect a drop in respirometer oxygen concentration and presumably correspond to exhalation by the bird. Similarly, the drop in V̇O2 would therefore correspond to inspiration. If this is so, then the first post-dive exhalation occurs about 0.5 s after surfacing.

Fig. 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 9.

Mean VO2 observed after long dives of four tufted ducks. Data were smoothed by taking a three-point moving average. As well as the general exponential decrease in VO2 over the first 15 s post surface, there is a cyclical aspect to the data. This is highlighted subjectively on the graph (black bars), and is believed to correspond to expiration (peak) and inspiration (trough), occurring at frequencies similar to those recorded by Butler and Woakes (1979) in a diving pochard (Aythya ferina). The arrow indicates the point of inflection for biphasic uptake.

Butler and Woakes (1979) studied the respiratory patterns of diving pochards (Aythya ferina) before and after feeding dives using implanted tracheal thermistors. They reported that the respiratory frequency decreased from 54.5 breaths min-1., 1 s after surfacing to 42.7 breaths min-1 after 5 s and longer. If the respirometry system used in the present study was able to detect individual breaths, the rates of post-dive ventilation for the pochard are comparable to those from the present study. There is clearly a change in respiratory frequency after the third exhalation, according to Fig. 9. The first three expiration markers cover approx. 2.5 respiratory cycles in 2.25 s, implying a respiratory frequency of 66.7 breaths min-1. The next five markers cover approx. 4.75 cycles in 8.75 s, a respiratory frequency of 32.5 breaths min-1. The biphasic inflection point, at 3.3 s, occurs in between the third and fourth expirations. Thus, the biphasic nature of oxygen uptake following longer dives may be explained by a fairly sudden slowing of ventilation rate after the first three exhalations. This agrees with reports of other diving vertebrate species reloading their oxygen stores and removing carbon dioxide after dives (Butler and Jones, 1997). However, not all air-breathing divers exhibit the same respiratory patterns as avian species. Weddell seals show a steady decline in minute volume (volume of air inhaled per minute) during the first 5 min post-dive, and beyond this point the decline decays to a constant (Kooyman et al., 1971).

A hypothesis borne from the assumptions on avian anatomy by Walton et al. (1998) is that the break point of the oxygen uptake curve would occur just after the second post-surface expiration, since this would be the first opportunity for the inspired gases to diffuse into the blood and tissues through the paleopulmonic parabronchi. The biphasic point of inflection is at 3.3 s for long duration dives, whereas the second exhalation occurs at 1.5 s (Fig. 9). This is further evidence that the biphasic uptake that occurs during longer dives in the tufted duck is probably associated with a change in respiratory frequency, rather than as a consequence of the anatomy of its respiratory system.

List of symbols
a1, a2, b1
linear components of the broken stick model
C
intersecting point of inflection
Ox1, Ox2
fractional concentrations of oxygen at times t1, t2 after leaving the chamber
Oxamb
fractional concentration of (ambient) oxygen entering the respirometer
Q̇
flow rate out of the respirometer (ml s-1)
t
time
t1 and t2
start and finish, respectively, of the measured period of time
ts
optimal surface time
tT
travel time
V
respirometer volume
V̇ O2
rate of oxygen uptake
V̇ O2
total oxygen consumption between times t1 and t2
VO2d
total oxygen consumed during the dive

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to Peter Frappell for his comments on the respirometry. This work was supported by a BBSRC postgraduate studentship and a NERC postgraduate studentship.

  • © The Company of Biologists Limited 2002

References

  1. ↵
    Bentley, J. P. (1983). Signals and noise in measurement systems. In Principles of Measurement Systems, pp. 88-114. London: Longman.
  2. ↵
    Bevan, R. M. and Butler, P. J. (1992). The effects of temperature on the oxygen consumption, heart rate and deep body temperature during diving in the tufted duck Aythya fuligula. J. Exp. Biol. 163,139 -151.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Boggs, D. F., Butler, P. J. and Wallace, S. E. (1998). Differential air sac pressures in diving tufted ducks (Aythya fuligula). J. Exp. Biol. 201,2665 -2668.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Bretz, W. L. and Schmidt-Nielsen, K. (1971). Bird respiration: flow patterns in the duck lung. J. Exp. Biol. 54,103 -118.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Butler, J. P., Banzett, R. B. and Fredberg, J. J. (1988). Inspiratory valving in avian bronchi: aerodynamic considerations. Resp. Physiol. 72,241 -256.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Butler, P. J. and Woakes, A. J. (1979). Changes in heart rate and respiratory frequency during natural behaviour of ducks, with particular reference to diving. J. Exp. Biol. 79,283 -300.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Butler, P. J. and Jones, D. R. (1997). Physiology of diving of birds and mammals. Physiol. Rev. 77,837 -899.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Carbone, C. and Houston, A. I. (1994). Patterns in the diving behaviour of the pochard, Aythya ferina: a test of an optimality model. Anim. Behav. 48,457 -465.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    Carbone, C. and Houston, A. I. (1996). The optimal allocation of time over the dive cycle: an approach based on aerobic and anaerobic respiration. Anim. Behav. 51,1247 -1255.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  10. ↵
    Carbone, C., De Leeuw, J. J. and Houston, A. I. (1996). Adjustments in the diving time budgets of the tufted duck and the pochard: is there evidence for a mix of metabolic pathways? Anim. Behav. 51,1257 -1268.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor. Pop. Biol. 9, 129-136.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  12. ↵
    Croll, D. A., Gaston, A. J., Burger, A. E. and Konnoff, D. (1992). Foraging behaviour and physiological adaptations in thick billed murres. Ecology 73,344 -356.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    Houston, A. I. (2000). Nonmonotonic dive to surface ratios: comments on Walton et al. (1998). Anim. Behav. 59,F13 -F15.
  14. ↵
    Houston, A. I. and Carbone, C. (1992). The optimal allocation of time during the diving cycle. Behav. Ecol. 3,233 -262.
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    Houston, A. I. and McNamara, J. M. (1985). A general theory of central place foraging for single-prey loaders. Theor. Pop. Biol. 28,233 -262.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    Kacelnik, A. (1984). Central place foraging in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch residence time. J. Anim. Ecol. 53,283 -299.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    Keijer, E. and Butler, P. J. (1982). Volumes of the respiratory and circulatory systems in the tufted and mallard ducks. J. Exp. Biol. 101,213 -220.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Koops, W. J. and Grossman, M. (1993). Multiphasic allometry. Growth, Dev. Aging 57,183 -192.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    Kooyman, G. L., Kerem, D. H., Campbell, W. B. and Wright, J. J. (1971). Pulmonary function in freely diving weddell seals. Resp. Physiol. 12,271 -282.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  20. ↵
    Kooyman, G. L. and Ponganis, P. J. (1998). The physiological basis of diving to depth: birds and mammals. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 60,19 -33.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  21. ↵
    Kramer, D. L. (1988). The behavioural ecology of air breathing by aquatic animals. Can. J. Zool. 66, 89-94.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    Lea, S. E. G., Daley, C., Boddington, P. J. C. and Morison, V. (1996). Diving patterns in shags and cormorants (Phalacrocorax): tests of an optimal breathing model. Ibis 138,391 -398.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Lessells, C. M. and Stephens, D. W. (1983). Central place foraging: single prey loader again. Anim. Behav. 31,238 -243.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    Magnusdottir, M. L. and Einarsson, A. (1990). Diving times of ducks at Lake Mývatn. Nattutuverndarrao 23,79 -92.
  25. ↵
    Mori, Y. (1998). The optimal patch use in divers: Optimal time budget and the number of dive cycles during bout. J. Theor. Biol. 190,187 -199.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. ↵
    Mori, Y. (1999). The optimal time allocation of time and respiratory metabolism over the dive cycle. Behav. Ecol. 10,155 -160.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    Orians, G. H. and Pearson, N. E. (1979). On the theory of central place foraging. In Analysis of Ecological Systems (ed. D. J. Horn, R. D. Mitchell and G. R. Stairs), pp.155 -177. Columbus: Ohio State University Press.
  28. ↵
    Ponganis, P. J., Kooyman, G. L., Van Dam, R. and LeMaho, Y. (1999). Physiological responses of king penguins during simulated diving to 136 m depth. J. Exp. Biol. 202,2819 -2822.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  29. ↵
    Powell, F. L. (2000). Respiration. In Sturkie's Avian Physiology, 5th edn. (ed. P. D. Sturkie), pp. 233-264. Springer-Verlag, New York.
  30. ↵
    Ruxton, G. D., Walton, P. and Monaghan, P. (2000). The shape of oxygen recovery curves: reply to Houston (2000). Anim. Behav. 59,F16 .
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    Scheid, P. (1979). Mechanisms of gas exchange in bird lungs. Rev. Physiol. Biochem. Pharmacol. 86,137 -186.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  32. ↵
    Seber, G. A. F. (1977). Linear Regression Analysis, pp. 204-209. London, New York: Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  33. ↵
    Slater, P. J. B. and Lester, N. P. (1982). Minimising errors in splitting behaviour into bouts. Behaviour 79,153 -161.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  34. ↵
    Stephenson, R. (1994). Diving energetics in lesser scaup (Aythya affinis, Eyton). J. Exp. Biol. 190,155 -178.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Stephenson, R. (1995). Respiratory and plumage gas volumes in unrestrained diving ducks (Aythya affinis). Respir. Physiol. 100,129 -137.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Stephenson, R., Butler, P. J. and Woakes, A. J. (1986). Diving behaviour and heart rate in tufted ducks (Aythya fuligula). J. Exp. Biol. 126,341 -359.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Thompson, D., Hiby, A. R. and Fedak, M. A. (1993). How fast should I swim? Behavioural implications of diving physiology. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 66,349 -368.
    OpenUrl
  38. Wallace, S. E. (1998). Foraging energetics of diving ducks. PhD thesis, Birmingham University.
  39. ↵
    Walton, P., Ruxton, G. D. and Monaghan, P. (1998). Avian diving, respiratory physiology and the marginal value theorem. Anim. Behav. 56,165 -174.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. ↵
    Woakes, A. J. and Butler, P. J. (1983). Swimming and diving in the tufted ducks Aythya fuligula, with particular reference to heart rate and gas exchange. J. Exp. Biol. 107,313 -329.
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    Ydenberg, R. C. and Clark, C. W. (1989). Aerobiosis and anaerobiosis during diving by western grebes: An optimal foraging approach. J. Theor. Biol. 139,437 -449.
    OpenUrl
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Experimental Biology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Oxygen uptake during post dive recovery in a diving bird Aythya fuligula: implications for optimal foraging models
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Experimental Biology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Experimental Biology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
Oxygen uptake during post dive recovery in a diving bird Aythya fuligula: implications for optimal foraging models
Roland Parkes, Lewis G. Halsey, Anthony J. Woakes, Roger L. Holder, Patrick J. Butler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2002 205: 3945-3954;
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
Oxygen uptake during post dive recovery in a diving bird Aythya fuligula: implications for optimal foraging models
Roland Parkes, Lewis G. Halsey, Anthony J. Woakes, Roger L. Holder, Patrick J. Butler
Journal of Experimental Biology 2002 205: 3945-3954;

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • SUMMARY
    • Introduction
    • Materials and methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Info & metrics
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Angling gear avoidance learning in juvenile red sea bream: evidence from individual-based experiments
  • Tactile active sensing in an insect plant pollinator
  • Omega-3 fatty acids accelerate fledging in an avian marine predator: a potential role of cognition
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLE

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Biology Open

Advertisement

Welcome to JEB’s new Editor Monica Daley

We are pleased to welcome Monica Daley to JEB’s Editorial team. Monica has had a long association with JEB before taking up her new role, overseeing peer review of neuromuscular physiology, terrestrial biomechanics and integrative physiology of locomotion.


In the field with Robyn Hetem

Continuing our fieldwork series, Robyn Hetem reflects on working with species ranging from aardvark to zebra, and the impact COVID-19 has had on fieldwork.


Read & Publish participation continues to grow

“It is particularly encouraging for early career researchers, as it allows them to display their research globally without the need to find costs to cover the open access option.”

Professor Fernando Montealegre-Z (University of Lincoln) shares his experience of publishing Open Access as part of our growing Read & Publish initiative. We now have over 150 institutions in 15 countries and four library consortia taking part – find out more and view our full list of participating institutions.


Nocturnal reef residents have deep-sea-like eyes

Fanny de Busserolles and colleagues from The University of Queensland have discovered that the eyes of nocturnal reef fish have multibank retinas, layers of photoreceptors, similar to the eyes of deep-sea fish that live in dim light conditions.


Mechanisms underlying gut microbiota–host interactions in insects

In their Review, Konstantin Schmidt and Philipp Engel summarise recent findings about the mechanisms involved in gut colonisation and the provisioning of beneficial effects in gut microbiota–insect symbiosis.

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Special issues
  • Subject collections
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About JEB
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Travelling Fellowships
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists
  • Journal news

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access
  • Outstanding paper prize
  • Biology Open transfer

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact JEB
  • Subscriptions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

 Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992