Cranial kinesis is an important feature in avian feeding behaviour and involves the transmission of quadrate movement to the upper bill by the Pterygoid–Palatinum Complex (PPC). The PPC in Palaeognathae is remarkably different from that found in Neognathae. In this study we analyse whether the special morphology of the PPC is an adaptation to the feeding behaviour of the Palaeognathae. Behavioural analyses of the rhea Rhea americana showed that the feeding behaviour of the rhea is typical `Catch and Throw' behaviour, independent of the size of the food item. Drinking is achieved by a scooping movement followed by a low-amplitude tip-up phase. During feeding rhynchokinetic movements of the upper bill were observed. However, cranial kinesis was limited and may differ from rhynchokinesis in neognathes as a clear bending zone seemed absent. Since the movement patterns are considered very similar to the basic feeding behaviour in neognathous birds it is concluded that the specific morphology of the PPC is not the result of specific functional demands from palaeognathous feeding behaviour.

Since Merrem (1813)separated the Palaeognathae from all other birds, the taxon Palaeognathae has caused many disputes among ornithologists. One of the most important characters that separates the Palaeognathae from all other modern birds(Neognathae) is the dromaeognathous (= palaeognathous) palate, first described by Huxley (1867). It is not only the `palate' that is different from that found in Neognathae, but a whole complex of morphological characters, which includes structures such as the pterygoid, quadrate and vomer (see McDowell, 1948; Bock, 1963; Gussekloo and Zweers, 1999). The character set of the jaw mechanism that discriminates the Palaeognathae from the Neognathae will be referred to as the palaeognathous Pterygoid–Palate Complex (palaeognathous PPC; Gussekloo and Zweers, 1999). The mechanical function of this PPC in neognathous birds is well known. The PPC participates in the movement of the upper bill(Bock, 1964). Upper bill movement is induced by rostrad rotation of the quadrate, which pushes both the lateral jugal bars and the medial pterygoid–palate bar forward. Each bar transfers its forces and movement onto the premaxilla. The forward movement of the premaxilla results in an upward rotation of the upper bill around a hinge,either in the nasal–frontal area (prokinesis) or in the rostral part of the bill (rhynchokinesis), depending on the position of a flexible zone. In the palaeognathous birds it is assumed that a large flexible zone is present in the centre of the upper bill (Fig. 1, Zusi, 1984). The pterygoid–palate bar and the quadrate are of great importance for cranial kinesis since these bars transfer the forces, and the muscles for the movement of the upper bill attach to these elements.

Although many authors have used the palaeognathous PPC for systematic purposes (Fürbringer,1888; Gadow, 1892; Beddard, 1898; McDowell, 1948; de Beer, 1956; Bock, 1963), studies on the function of the system are very limited. Most previous analyses assumed that the special morphology of the PPC in Palaeognathae is related to rhynchokinesis (Hofer, 1954; Simonetta, 1960; Bock, 1963), mainly because of the osteology of the PPC, the flexibility of the dorsal and ventral bars of the upper bill, and the incomplete ossification of the lateral bar(Zusi, 1984). The movement pattern of the PPC during bill opening has been measured, and showed very little difference between Palaeognathae and Neognathae(Gussekloo et al., 2001). These studies, however, were done on either osteological specimens or head preparations and it is currently unknown if movement of the PPC and rhynchokinesis actually occurs in living palaeognathous birds.

In the present study we did a functional analysis of the feeding behaviour of the greater rhea Rhea americana (L.) in order to elucidate the function and origin of the palaeognathous PPC. Several hypotheses can be postulated about the evolution of the special PPC morphology in the Palaeognathae. Our main hypothesis is that the specific palaeognathous morphology of the PPC is an adaptation to selective forces that act on the PPC in palaeognathous birds, but not in neognathous birds. Since the function of the PPC is the transfer of forces and movements during upper bill movement, it is assumed that these selective forces must be related to bill movement. Feeding behaviour is considered the strongest selection force acting on bill movement, and therefore on the PPC. Other behaviours such as vocalisation,preening and social behaviour are considered to have little effect on the osteology of the bills. To investigate whether differences in selection forces on bill movement are present, bill movement of a typical palaeognathous bird during feeding will be described and compared with a previously described general neognathous-feeding pattern(Zweers et al., 1994). If differences are found in the feeding behaviour it may be possible to infer which selective forces resulted in the differences in PPC morphology. If no differences in feeding pattern can be found between Neognathae and Palaeognathae it must be concluded that no different selective forces act on the PPC during feeding and an alternative hypothesis about the origin of the difference in morphology between Neognathae and Palaeognathae must be postulated.

Fig. 1.

Types of cranial kinesis in birds. (A) Prokinesis, (B) distal rhynchokinesis, (C) central rhynchokinesis. Skull outlines are given in lateral view. Black triangles indicate the main area of rotation, multiple triangles in a single element indicate the boundaries of an bending zone. The triangle in A indicates the nasal–frontal articulation. Arrows indicate the lateral (nasal) bar of the upper bill, which is incomplete in C. Ligament is shown in gray. (Adapted from Zusi,1984.)

Fig. 1.

Types of cranial kinesis in birds. (A) Prokinesis, (B) distal rhynchokinesis, (C) central rhynchokinesis. Skull outlines are given in lateral view. Black triangles indicate the main area of rotation, multiple triangles in a single element indicate the boundaries of an bending zone. The triangle in A indicates the nasal–frontal articulation. Arrows indicate the lateral (nasal) bar of the upper bill, which is incomplete in C. Ligament is shown in gray. (Adapted from Zusi,1984.)

The greater rhea Rhea americana (L.), a middle-sized palaeognathous bird from South-America, was chosen as representative for the Palaeognathae. This species has a general palaeognathous PPC configuration(McDowell, 1948) and its natural history and behaviour are well known (Raikow, 1968, 1969; Bruning, 1974; Martella et al., 1995, 1996; Reboreda and Fernandez, 1997). For the analysis two animals, one male and one female, were trained to feed on several food types within the experimental set-up. For this analysis, feeding will include only the behavioural elements from picking-up the food item until swallowing. All phases prior to the picking-up for intra-oral transport are considered a part of food-acquisition.

The feeding behaviour of the birds was recorded using video imaging (25 frames s–1). The recordings were made in an experimental set-up in which a lateral view and a frontal view of the bird were obtained in the same frame using a mirror situated in front of the bird at an angle of 45°. The birds had to approach the feeding arena through a small corridor,ensuring a good lateral position of the bird with respect to the camera. Behind the bird, from the camera's viewpoint, a grid (2 cm×2 cm squares)was placed to make scaling possible. The films were analysed, frame-by-frame,by digitising the position of several points on the upper and lower bill relative to the standard grid (Fig. 2). Prior to the feeding analysis the position of the bending zones was determined through manipulating osteological specimens. The positions found were compared to previous descriptions(Hofer, 1954; Simonetta, 1960; Bock, 1963; Zusi, 1984) and used to determine the position of points for digitising. In addition to these points on the bills, some reference points on the skull of the bird were also digitised (Fig. 2). From the complete set of digitised points a number of distances and angles was calculated (Table 1). The accuracy of the calculated distances and angles was determined on the basis of the variation in a standard measurement calculated as the distance between two digitised points of the reference grid. The standard error in this distance measurement was approximately 0.08 mm. The standard error in digitising a point was therefore approximately 0.04 mm in each direction. The errors for points were used to calculate the error for angles, which was dependent on the distance between the points and the angle between lines. The standard error of the mean angle of two parallel lines both of 4 cm (a typical length used in our analyses) is just under 0.5°. For each time point the mean value and 95% confidence intervals of the behavioural parameters were calculated and used to describe the mean behavioural pattern. The same data were also used to test if cranial kinesis is present in palaeognathous birds.

Fig. 2.

Digitised points in each frame of the feeding scenes. (1) Rostral edge of the eye, (2) caudal edge of the eye, (3) centre of the ear, (4) centre of the food item, (5) upper bill, near the bill tip (ventral edge), (6) upper bill,rostral of the bending zone (ventral edge), (7) upper bill, caudal of the bending zone (ventral edge), (8) upper bill, caudal part (ventral edge), (9)upper bill, most rostral point with feathers, (10) lower bill, near the bill tip (dorsal edge), (11) lower bill, rostral of the bending zone (dorsal edge),(12) lower bill, caudal of the bending zone (dorsal edge), (13) lower bill,caudal part (dorsal edge), (14) throat, near end rhamphotheca, (15) throat,near end lower jaw, (16) throat, 2 cm below marker 15. In addition to the points shown, three standard points on the background grid were digitised to determine horizontal and vertical axes and to scale the images.

Fig. 2.

Digitised points in each frame of the feeding scenes. (1) Rostral edge of the eye, (2) caudal edge of the eye, (3) centre of the ear, (4) centre of the food item, (5) upper bill, near the bill tip (ventral edge), (6) upper bill,rostral of the bending zone (ventral edge), (7) upper bill, caudal of the bending zone (ventral edge), (8) upper bill, caudal part (ventral edge), (9)upper bill, most rostral point with feathers, (10) lower bill, near the bill tip (dorsal edge), (11) lower bill, rostral of the bending zone (dorsal edge),(12) lower bill, caudal of the bending zone (dorsal edge), (13) lower bill,caudal part (dorsal edge), (14) throat, near end rhamphotheca, (15) throat,near end lower jaw, (16) throat, 2 cm below marker 15. In addition to the points shown, three standard points on the background grid were digitised to determine horizontal and vertical axes and to scale the images.

Table 1.

Calculated parameters used in the kinematic analysis

No.ParameterDescription
Standard measure Measured on the reference grid (mm) 
Gape Distance between upper and lower bill tip (mm) 
Distance food Distance between the food item and the upper bill tip (mm) 
X position head Relative horizontal position of the head (ear) (mm) 
Y position head Elevation of the head (ear) above the ground (mm) 
Flexion in nasal—frontal hinge Angle between cranium and caudal part of the upper bill (deg.) 
Flexion halfway the upper bill Angle between the caudal and rostral part of the bill (deg.) 
Opening lower bill Angle between the cranium and lower bill (deg.) 
Depression of the throat Distance between the cranium (ear) and oropharynx floor near the larynx (mm) 
No.ParameterDescription
Standard measure Measured on the reference grid (mm) 
Gape Distance between upper and lower bill tip (mm) 
Distance food Distance between the food item and the upper bill tip (mm) 
X position head Relative horizontal position of the head (ear) (mm) 
Y position head Elevation of the head (ear) above the ground (mm) 
Flexion in nasal—frontal hinge Angle between cranium and caudal part of the upper bill (deg.) 
Flexion halfway the upper bill Angle between the caudal and rostral part of the bill (deg.) 
Opening lower bill Angle between the cranium and lower bill (deg.) 
Depression of the throat Distance between the cranium (ear) and oropharynx floor near the larynx (mm) 

The data on head displacement were used to determine maximum velocities and accelerations of the head during feeding. The complete trajectory of the head was determined by interpolation to 250 points s–1 using a cubic-spline interpolation technique. The spline interpolation technique was used under the assumption that head movements follow a gradual and symmetric path around the points of change of direction. Behavioural observations confirm these assumptions. The acceleration data, in combination with the mass of the head (estimated from the head mass in other individuals) were used to determine the forces acting on the head.

A range of food types was offered (Table 2), varying in size between 4 mm and 35 mm in length. At least five items of each food type were analysed for each bird. Large apples were only eaten by the male and only on three occasions. Drinking cycles were observed in both individuals, but only seven cycles could be analysed.

Table 2.

Approximate dimensions of offered food types

No.Food typeDimensions (mm)
Apple large 35×35×35 
Apple small 25×25×25 
Pellets 10×10×25 
Seeds 4×3×8 
Water (drinking) 
No.Food typeDimensions (mm)
Apple large 35×35×35 
Apple small 25×25×25 
Pellets 10×10×25 
Seeds 4×3×8 
Water (drinking) 

To investigate the diversity and variability of the feeding behaviour, a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to describe the variation in feeding behaviour due to different food-types. The PCA, with Varimax rotation,was based on the correlation matrix of characters. The characters were obtained from the movement patterns of the different head elements involved in feeding (Table 3; see also Figs 5, 6). Differences in principal component scores were determined using an analysis of variance (ANOVA).

Table 3.

Measured parameters

GroupNo.ParameterDescription
Gape Gape 1 Maximum gape during the approach 
 Gape 2 Maximum gape during `Catch and Throw' 
 Gape level Mean gape between Gape 1 and 2 
 Gape level s.dStandard deviation of no. 3 (indicator for repositioning) 
 Gape period Time between Gape 1 and 2 
 Gape 2 Moment Time from start to Gape 2 
Lower bill Lower bill 1 Maximum depression of the lower bill during the approach 
 Lower bill 2 Maximum depression of the lower bill during the `Catch and Throw' 
 Lower bill level Mean depression of the lower bill between Lower bill 1 and 2 
 10 Lower bill s.dStandard deviation of no. 9 (indicator for repositioning) 
 11 Lower bill period Time between lower bill 1 and 2 
 12 Lower bill 2 moment Time from start to lower bill 2 
Prokinesis 13 Prokinesis at Gape 1 Angle around nasal—frontal hinge at Gape 1 
 14 Prokinesis at Gape 2 Angle around nasal—frontal hinge at Gape 2 
 15 Prokinesis level Mean angle around nasal—frontal hinge between Gape 1 and Gape 2 
 16 Prokinesis level s.dStandard deviation of no. 15 (indicator for constancy of kinesis) 
Rhynchokinesis 17 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 Angle around bending zone in the upper bill at Gape 1 
 18 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 Angle around bending zone in the upper bill at Gape 2 
 19 Rhynchokinesis level Mean angle around bending zone in the upper bill between the moment of Gape 1 and Gape 2 
 20 Rhynchokinesis level s.dStandard deviation of no. 19 (indicator for constancy of kinesis) 
Food 21 Food level Mean distance between the cranium and the food-item between the moment of grasping and swallowing 
 22 Food level s.dStandard deviation of no. 21 (indicator for inter-oral transport other than`Catch and Throw') 
 23 Food period Duration of holding the food item 
 24 Food min moment Moment of release of the food item in the `Catch and Throw' 
Head Y 25 Head elevation period Time between minimum and maximum elevation of the head 
 26 Difference head elevation Maximum distance of head elevation 
 27 Max head elevation moment Moment of maximal head elevation 
Head X 28 Head X period Time between minimum and maximum horizontal displacement of the head 
 29 Difference head X Maximum distance of horizontal head displacement 
 30 Min head X moment Moment of minimal horizontal extension of the neck (head closest to the body) 
Neck 31 Neck neck period Time between maximal and minimal flexion of the neck 
 32 Difference neck neck Difference in angle between maximal and minimal flexion 
 33 Neck neck moment Moment of minimum neck flexion 
Throat 34 Throat period Period between minimum and maximum throat depression 
 35 Difference throat Distance between minimum and maximum throat depression 
 36 Throat moment Moment of maximum throat depression 
GroupNo.ParameterDescription
Gape Gape 1 Maximum gape during the approach 
 Gape 2 Maximum gape during `Catch and Throw' 
 Gape level Mean gape between Gape 1 and 2 
 Gape level s.dStandard deviation of no. 3 (indicator for repositioning) 
 Gape period Time between Gape 1 and 2 
 Gape 2 Moment Time from start to Gape 2 
Lower bill Lower bill 1 Maximum depression of the lower bill during the approach 
 Lower bill 2 Maximum depression of the lower bill during the `Catch and Throw' 
 Lower bill level Mean depression of the lower bill between Lower bill 1 and 2 
 10 Lower bill s.dStandard deviation of no. 9 (indicator for repositioning) 
 11 Lower bill period Time between lower bill 1 and 2 
 12 Lower bill 2 moment Time from start to lower bill 2 
Prokinesis 13 Prokinesis at Gape 1 Angle around nasal—frontal hinge at Gape 1 
 14 Prokinesis at Gape 2 Angle around nasal—frontal hinge at Gape 2 
 15 Prokinesis level Mean angle around nasal—frontal hinge between Gape 1 and Gape 2 
 16 Prokinesis level s.dStandard deviation of no. 15 (indicator for constancy of kinesis) 
Rhynchokinesis 17 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 Angle around bending zone in the upper bill at Gape 1 
 18 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 Angle around bending zone in the upper bill at Gape 2 
 19 Rhynchokinesis level Mean angle around bending zone in the upper bill between the moment of Gape 1 and Gape 2 
 20 Rhynchokinesis level s.dStandard deviation of no. 19 (indicator for constancy of kinesis) 
Food 21 Food level Mean distance between the cranium and the food-item between the moment of grasping and swallowing 
 22 Food level s.dStandard deviation of no. 21 (indicator for inter-oral transport other than`Catch and Throw') 
 23 Food period Duration of holding the food item 
 24 Food min moment Moment of release of the food item in the `Catch and Throw' 
Head Y 25 Head elevation period Time between minimum and maximum elevation of the head 
 26 Difference head elevation Maximum distance of head elevation 
 27 Max head elevation moment Moment of maximal head elevation 
Head X 28 Head X period Time between minimum and maximum horizontal displacement of the head 
 29 Difference head X Maximum distance of horizontal head displacement 
 30 Min head X moment Moment of minimal horizontal extension of the neck (head closest to the body) 
Neck 31 Neck neck period Time between maximal and minimal flexion of the neck 
 32 Difference neck neck Difference in angle between maximal and minimal flexion 
 33 Neck neck moment Moment of minimum neck flexion 
Throat 34 Throat period Period between minimum and maximum throat depression 
 35 Difference throat Distance between minimum and maximum throat depression 
 36 Throat moment Moment of maximum throat depression 

In addition to an analysis of the structure of the general feeding and drinking pattern, emphasis was laid on the presence of cranial kinesis, since many authors have coupled the morphology of the palaeognathous PPC directly to it. During all the observed feeding cycles both movement near the nasal–frontal area and movement in the upper bill were monitored to determine if cranial kinesis is present during normal feeding behaviour. Changes in angles were compared statistically using ANOVA to determine if bending actually occurs.

General feeding behaviour

When describing the feeding behaviour of the rhea, the same elements are used as in the general description for neognathous feeding used by Zweers et al. (1994). The first elements of the neognathous feeding behaviour are preliminary head fixation,preliminary head approach and final head fixation. None of these elements are observed in the rhea. After final fixation neognathous feeding behaviour continues with the following elements: (1) final head approach, (2) catch at jaw tips, (3) stationing and repositioning, (4) catch at jaw tips, (5)intra-oral transport (`Catch and Throw'), (6) intra-pharyngeal transport.

The general feeding sequence of the rhea (Figs 3A, 4) resembles this pattern: the bird approaches the food item while opening the bills and the food item is picked up. When the head hits the ground the acceleration of the head is approximately 11.30 m s–2 (a=11.30±6.57 m s–2, N=41). With an estimated head mass of 0.25 kg,the mean calculated impact force was 2.83 N and the maximum did not exceed 7.54 N (amax=30.17 m s–2).

Grasping the food is sometimes followed by repositioning behaviour. Repositioning occurs in the rhea more often when large rather than small food items were eaten.

When the food item was correctly positioned, a single `Catch and Throw'movement is used to transport the food particle into, or near to, the entrance of the oesophagus. A `Catch and Throw' movement starts when the food is fixed between the bills, the head is accelerated upward and slightly backward. Then the bills open and the head is suddenly moved forward. The accelerated food item continues to move upward while the head of the bird moves downward, which results in the transport of the food item. The palaeognathous single `Catch and Throw' movement is accompanied by a large gape and a large depression of the tongue. This depression results in an enlargement of the buccal cavity,which facilitates transport of the food item into the caudal part of the oropharynx. No tongue movement was observed other than the one resulting in the depression of the mouth floor. The limited functions of the tongue in feeding are reflected in the morphology of the tongue, which is relatively small with no remarkable features (Fig. 5). The oropharynx itself also shows very little remarkable characters that might indicate feeding patterns other than `Catch and Throw'behaviour.

Fig. 3.

Outline drawings of feeding behaviour (A) and scoop drinking behaviour (B)of the greater rhea Rhea americana. Horizontal lines represent ground level in A or water level in B.

Fig. 3.

Outline drawings of feeding behaviour (A) and scoop drinking behaviour (B)of the greater rhea Rhea americana. Horizontal lines represent ground level in A or water level in B.

General drinking behaviour

The rhea uses two different types of drinking behaviour, depending on the area of water available to drink from. The preferred method of drinking can be described as scoop drinking followed by a low-amplitude tip-up phase(Fig. 3B). During drinking the bird opens the bill, inserts it into the water, and with a forward scooping motion of the head the lower bill is filled with water. The bill is then closed and the head is elevated until the neck is almost completely stretched,while the head itself is in a horizontal position. Finally, the water is transported into the oesophagus by a slight elevation of the bill tips and a retraction of the tongue. In some cases small horizontal `Catch and Throw'movements are used to transport the water more caudally in the oropharynx just prior to swallowing.

Fig. 4.

A characteristic feeding cycle of the greater rhea Rhea americana. The vertical broken lines indicate characteristic moments in the feeding cycle. In chronological order: (A) picking up the food item (grasp), (B)maximum gape during a repositioning cycle and (C) maximum gape during the transport phase. The graphs show the gape, the vertical (Y) and horizontal (X) positions of the head, the distance between the food item and the bill tips, the depression of the throat and the flexion of the most rostral part of the neck.

Fig. 4.

A characteristic feeding cycle of the greater rhea Rhea americana. The vertical broken lines indicate characteristic moments in the feeding cycle. In chronological order: (A) picking up the food item (grasp), (B)maximum gape during a repositioning cycle and (C) maximum gape during the transport phase. The graphs show the gape, the vertical (Y) and horizontal (X) positions of the head, the distance between the food item and the bill tips, the depression of the throat and the flexion of the most rostral part of the neck.

When the size of the water surface limits the scooping movement, the rhea uses a drinking technique that is very similar to pecking behaviour. The bill is opened and inserted almost vertically into the water, the bill is then closed and in a single head jerk the water is accelerated vertically, the bill is opened and the water is transported to the back of the oropharynx. Since this behaviour strongly resembles pecking, and is not the basic drinking behaviour, it was not included in this analysis.

Quantitative differences between food types

To characterise the movement patterns quantitatively 36 parameters were chosen (Table 3, Figs 4, 6) and analysed using a Principal Component Analysis (PCA). The first three principal components of the PCA (PC1–3), based on the characters of the feeding and drinking behaviours described 63% of the total variance. An analysis of variance(ANOVA) over the principal component scores was used to determine the main differences between individuals/sexes and food types. None of the first three principal components showed a difference between individuals/sexes (d.f.=47,PC1: F=0.264, P=NS; PC2: F=0.198, P=NS;PC3: F=0.240, P=NS, where NS=not significant) and therefore the data from both individuals were combined. It is clear from the plot of the first principal component (PC1) against the second principal component (PC2)that drinking behaviour is remarkably different from feeding behaviour(Fig. 7, Table 4). The first principal component describes the absence of the second gape movement (`Catch and Throw'movement), differences in neck movement (duration of the neck cycle) and the duration of the total feeding cycle (Table 5). The second principal component describes differences in food manipulation by the bills, such as position of the food item between the bills, depression of the lower bill and upper bill kinesis. To investigate the differences between food types without the large distorting effect of drinking, the PCA was repeated using the four types of feeding behaviour only. In this analysis 65% of the variance was explained by the first three principal components. To test whether there are significant differences between food types, a one-way ANOVA over the first three principal components scores was used. Differences between the food types were tested using a t-test with Bonferroni correction. There are significant differences between food types on both the first and third principal component (d.f.=40,PC1: F=28.678, P<0.001; PC2: F=0.365, P=NS; PC3: F=3.628, P<0.05). It is clear that PC1 describes the effect of food size (Fig. 8). The differences on the first principal component represent mainly the effect of the duration of the movement for each food type (e.g. gape period, head elevation period, food period, lower bill period), the size of the first gape (Gape 1) and the elevation of the head (difference head Y; Table 6). All these parameters increase with an increase of the size of the food type, which indicates that the movement pattern of food uptake is relatively constant and that only the duration, mainly the effect of repositioning, and amplitude of the movement differ between different food sizes. The change in movement described by PC1 becomes smaller when the size of the food items increases. A difference on PC1 is only found between seeds and all other food types (food type 1 vs 2, 3 and 4, t-test, Bonferroni correction, P<0.001).

Table 4.

Mean values per food type for all characters used in the principal component analysis

Food
GroupNo.Parameter1 Apple large2 Apple small3 Pellets4 Seeds5 Water
Gape Gape 1 (cm) 2.80 2.31 2.09 1.49 4.66 
 Gape 2 (cm) 7.53 5.47 5.24 2.43 
 Gape level (cm) 3.46 1.88 1.67 0.52 0.37 
 Gape level s.d. (cm) 0.74 0.86 0.72 0.55 0.90 
 Gape period (frames) 18.33 14.73 17.18 7.86 21.43 
 Gape 2 moment (frames) 17.67 13.82 15.73 6.36 19.71 
Lower bill Lower bill 1 (deg.) 32.37 42.94 41.23 41.16 40.20 
 Lower bill 2 (deg.) 58.70 55.60 55.46 45.98 
 Lower bill level (deg.) 39.06 38.50 39.74 32.74 25.56 
 10 Lower bill s.d. (deg.) 8.60 5.87 5.48 3.82 6.50 
 11 Lower bill period (frames) 18.67 14.27 17.27 7.79 21.14 
 12 Lower bill 2 moment (frames) 18.00 13.45 16.00 6.29 19.71 
Prokinesis 13 Prokinesis at Gape 1 (deg.) 20.58 21.94 21.83 23.42 13.53 
 14 Prokinesis at Gape 2 (deg.) 18.60 21.49 23.44 22.68 22.36 
 15 Prokinesis level (deg.) 21.00 22.29 22.50 25.37 17.19 
 16 Prokinesis level s.d. (deg.) 4.11 3.98 3.28 3.66 5.04 
Rhynchokinesis 17 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 (deg.) 6.12 6.19 8.92 4.29 -4.48 
 18 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 (deg.) 2.29 2.39 3.65 4.56 3.52 
 19 Rhynchokinesis level (deg.) 3.50 3.29 5.15 2.29 0.99 
 20 Rhynchokinesis level s.d. (deg.) 5.53 4.78 4.63 4.75 7.17 
Food 21 Food level (cm) 1.91 1.43 1.46 1.33 3.37 
 22 Food level s.d. (cm) 0.62 0.60 0.66 1.47 1.88 
 23 Food period (frames) 15.33 12.45 15.36 6.64 23.71 
 24 Food min moment (frames) -1.00 -0.27 -0.58 -1.14 -0.29 
Head Y 25 Head elevation period (frames) 17.67 12.90 15.45 6.64 20.43 
 26 Difference head elevation (cm) 25.03 21.71 21.24 15.23 84.92 
 27 Max head elevation moment (frames) 18.00 12.70 15.09 5.93 20.00 
Head X 28 Head X period (frames) 12.33 12.73 10.45 6.64 8.43 
 29 Difference head X (cm) 3.92 9.37 8.27 5.86 6.13 
 30 Min head X moment (frames) 16.00 11.18 15.09 5.36 7.86 
Neck 31 Neck neck period (frames) 16.00 12.45 15.55 5.69 5.43 
 32 Difference neck neck (deg.) 46.81 57.11 50.99 32.82 36.26 
 33 Neck neck moment (frames) 16.00 12.27 15.18 5.08 4.86 
Throat 34 Throat period (frames) 17.33 14.91 17.54 5.79 12.00 
 35 Difference throat (cm) 5.09 3.09 3.41 2.51 2.36 
 36 Throat moment (frames) 17.67 14.09 15.91 6.64 6.86 
Food
GroupNo.Parameter1 Apple large2 Apple small3 Pellets4 Seeds5 Water
Gape Gape 1 (cm) 2.80 2.31 2.09 1.49 4.66 
 Gape 2 (cm) 7.53 5.47 5.24 2.43 
 Gape level (cm) 3.46 1.88 1.67 0.52 0.37 
 Gape level s.d. (cm) 0.74 0.86 0.72 0.55 0.90 
 Gape period (frames) 18.33 14.73 17.18 7.86 21.43 
 Gape 2 moment (frames) 17.67 13.82 15.73 6.36 19.71 
Lower bill Lower bill 1 (deg.) 32.37 42.94 41.23 41.16 40.20 
 Lower bill 2 (deg.) 58.70 55.60 55.46 45.98 
 Lower bill level (deg.) 39.06 38.50 39.74 32.74 25.56 
 10 Lower bill s.d. (deg.) 8.60 5.87 5.48 3.82 6.50 
 11 Lower bill period (frames) 18.67 14.27 17.27 7.79 21.14 
 12 Lower bill 2 moment (frames) 18.00 13.45 16.00 6.29 19.71 
Prokinesis 13 Prokinesis at Gape 1 (deg.) 20.58 21.94 21.83 23.42 13.53 
 14 Prokinesis at Gape 2 (deg.) 18.60 21.49 23.44 22.68 22.36 
 15 Prokinesis level (deg.) 21.00 22.29 22.50 25.37 17.19 
 16 Prokinesis level s.d. (deg.) 4.11 3.98 3.28 3.66 5.04 
Rhynchokinesis 17 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 (deg.) 6.12 6.19 8.92 4.29 -4.48 
 18 Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 (deg.) 2.29 2.39 3.65 4.56 3.52 
 19 Rhynchokinesis level (deg.) 3.50 3.29 5.15 2.29 0.99 
 20 Rhynchokinesis level s.d. (deg.) 5.53 4.78 4.63 4.75 7.17 
Food 21 Food level (cm) 1.91 1.43 1.46 1.33 3.37 
 22 Food level s.d. (cm) 0.62 0.60 0.66 1.47 1.88 
 23 Food period (frames) 15.33 12.45 15.36 6.64 23.71 
 24 Food min moment (frames) -1.00 -0.27 -0.58 -1.14 -0.29 
Head Y 25 Head elevation period (frames) 17.67 12.90 15.45 6.64 20.43 
 26 Difference head elevation (cm) 25.03 21.71 21.24 15.23 84.92 
 27 Max head elevation moment (frames) 18.00 12.70 15.09 5.93 20.00 
Head X 28 Head X period (frames) 12.33 12.73 10.45 6.64 8.43 
 29 Difference head X (cm) 3.92 9.37 8.27 5.86 6.13 
 30 Min head X moment (frames) 16.00 11.18 15.09 5.36 7.86 
Neck 31 Neck neck period (frames) 16.00 12.45 15.55 5.69 5.43 
 32 Difference neck neck (deg.) 46.81 57.11 50.99 32.82 36.26 
 33 Neck neck moment (frames) 16.00 12.27 15.18 5.08 4.86 
Throat 34 Throat period (frames) 17.33 14.91 17.54 5.79 12.00 
 35 Difference throat (cm) 5.09 3.09 3.41 2.51 2.36 
 36 Throat moment (frames) 17.67 14.09 15.91 6.64 6.86 

Values are measurements relative to either the cranium or the reference grid; N=3 (large apple), 11 (small apple), 12 (pellets), 14 (seeds),7 (water).

Table 5.

Main parameters contributing to the first three principal components of the total analysis

CharacterPC1 (45%)PC2 (10%)PC3 (9%)
PC1    
    Throat moment 0.983 0.063 0.037 
    Gape 2 moment 0.981 0.081 0.054 
    Lower bill 2 moment 0.977 0.038 0.087 
    Food 2 moment 0.975 0.054 -0.012 
    Gape period 0.974 0.082 0.043 
    Head elevation moment 0.966 0.091 0.093 
    Head elevation period 0.965 0.068 0.077 
    Lower bill period 0.956 0.119 0.103 
    Food period 0.951 0.029 -0.029 
    Throat period 0.946 0.008 -0.069 
    Neck neck moment 0.911 0.196 0.178 
    Neck neck period 0.907 0.183 0.191 
    Min head X moment 0.881 0.200 0.090 
PC2    
    Food level 0.091 0.428 0.055 
    Gape level s.d0.274 0.422 0.592 
    Food level s.d-0.247 0.404 -0.047 
    Lower bill level 0.634 -0.435 -0.002 
    Lower bill at Gape 1 0.103 -0.565 -0.385 
    Prokinesis level -0.381 -0.628 -0.162 
    Prokinesis at Gape 2 0.023 -0.683 0.173 
    Prokinesis at Gape 1 -0.175 -0.714 0.102 
PC3    
    Throat displacement 0.362 0.123 0.672 
    Difference neck neck 0.417 -0.263 0.504 
    Lower bill level s.d0.416 0.397 0.486 
    Lower bill at Gape 2 0.571 -0.089 0.451 
    Difference head elevation 0.729 -0.064 0.443 
    Rhynchokinesis level s.d-0.043 -0.075 0.438 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 0.348 0.026 -0.410 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 -0.038 0.332 -0.679 
CharacterPC1 (45%)PC2 (10%)PC3 (9%)
PC1    
    Throat moment 0.983 0.063 0.037 
    Gape 2 moment 0.981 0.081 0.054 
    Lower bill 2 moment 0.977 0.038 0.087 
    Food 2 moment 0.975 0.054 -0.012 
    Gape period 0.974 0.082 0.043 
    Head elevation moment 0.966 0.091 0.093 
    Head elevation period 0.965 0.068 0.077 
    Lower bill period 0.956 0.119 0.103 
    Food period 0.951 0.029 -0.029 
    Throat period 0.946 0.008 -0.069 
    Neck neck moment 0.911 0.196 0.178 
    Neck neck period 0.907 0.183 0.191 
    Min head X moment 0.881 0.200 0.090 
PC2    
    Food level 0.091 0.428 0.055 
    Gape level s.d0.274 0.422 0.592 
    Food level s.d-0.247 0.404 -0.047 
    Lower bill level 0.634 -0.435 -0.002 
    Lower bill at Gape 1 0.103 -0.565 -0.385 
    Prokinesis level -0.381 -0.628 -0.162 
    Prokinesis at Gape 2 0.023 -0.683 0.173 
    Prokinesis at Gape 1 -0.175 -0.714 0.102 
PC3    
    Throat displacement 0.362 0.123 0.672 
    Difference neck neck 0.417 -0.263 0.504 
    Lower bill level s.d0.416 0.397 0.486 
    Lower bill at Gape 2 0.571 -0.089 0.451 
    Difference head elevation 0.729 -0.064 0.443 
    Rhynchokinesis level s.d-0.043 -0.075 0.438 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 1 0.348 0.026 -0.410 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 -0.038 0.332 -0.679 

For PC1, only parameters with loadings higher than 0.8 are selected; for PC2 and PC3, parameters with loadings higher than 0.4 are selected. Percentages indicate the explained variance on each PC.

Table 6.

Main parameters contributing to the first three principal components of the food type analysis

Principal component
ParameterPC1 (33%)PC2 (25%)PC3 (8%)
PC1    
    Gape period 0.970 0.168 0.017 
    Head elevation period 0.970 0.135 0.012 
    Head elevation moment 0.968 0.166 0.002 
    Food period 0.968 -0.062 0.019 
    Food 2 moment 0.965 0.136 0.027 
    Gape 2 moment 0.963 0.219 0.025 
    Lower bill 2 moment 0.963 0.209 0.059 
    Lower bill period 0.959 0.164 -0.023 
    Difference head elevation 0.741 -0.617 -0.145 
    Gape 1 0.734 -0.462 -0.088 
PC2    
    Gape 2 -0.052 0.914 0.051 
    Neck neck moment 0.361 0.885 0.072 
    Neck neck period 0.364 0.877 0.085 
    Throat moment 0.421 0.860 0.168 
    Gape level 0.200 0.829 -0.073 
    Lower bill at Gape 2 -0.482 0.804 0.203 
    Head X moment 0.514 0.748 0.017 
    Lower bill level -0.142 0.721 0.410 
PC3    
    Prokinesis at Gape 2 0.034 -0.154 0.744 
    Prokinesis at Gape 1 -0.429 0.143 0.639 
    Lower bill at Gape 1 -0.041 -0.004 0.513 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 -0.107 -0.474 
    Prokinesis level -0.635 0.089 0.469 
    Difference neck neck 0.230 0.390 0.437 
    Food level s.d0.028 -0.298 -0.427 
    Food level 0.457 -0.317 -0.419 
Principal component
ParameterPC1 (33%)PC2 (25%)PC3 (8%)
PC1    
    Gape period 0.970 0.168 0.017 
    Head elevation period 0.970 0.135 0.012 
    Head elevation moment 0.968 0.166 0.002 
    Food period 0.968 -0.062 0.019 
    Food 2 moment 0.965 0.136 0.027 
    Gape 2 moment 0.963 0.219 0.025 
    Lower bill 2 moment 0.963 0.209 0.059 
    Lower bill period 0.959 0.164 -0.023 
    Difference head elevation 0.741 -0.617 -0.145 
    Gape 1 0.734 -0.462 -0.088 
PC2    
    Gape 2 -0.052 0.914 0.051 
    Neck neck moment 0.361 0.885 0.072 
    Neck neck period 0.364 0.877 0.085 
    Throat moment 0.421 0.860 0.168 
    Gape level 0.200 0.829 -0.073 
    Lower bill at Gape 2 -0.482 0.804 0.203 
    Head X moment 0.514 0.748 0.017 
    Lower bill level -0.142 0.721 0.410 
PC3    
    Prokinesis at Gape 2 0.034 -0.154 0.744 
    Prokinesis at Gape 1 -0.429 0.143 0.639 
    Lower bill at Gape 1 -0.041 -0.004 0.513 
    Rhynchokinesis at Gape 2 -0.107 -0.474 
    Prokinesis level -0.635 0.089 0.469 
    Difference neck neck 0.230 0.390 0.437 
    Food level s.d0.028 -0.298 -0.427 
    Food level 0.457 -0.317 -0.419 

For PC1 and PC2, only parameters with loadings higher than 0.7 are selected; for PC3, parameters with loadings higher than 0.4 are selected. Percentages indicate the explained variance on each PC.

Fig. 5.

Oropharynx of the greater rhea Rhea americana. (A) Dorsal view of oropharynx floor. (B) Ventral view of oropharynx roof. a, rhamphotheca; b,tongue cushion; c, tongue base; d, opening of the trachea; e, larynx; f,esophagus; g, rhamphotheca; h, bony palate; i, soft palate; k, choana; m,vomer.

Fig. 5.

Oropharynx of the greater rhea Rhea americana. (A) Dorsal view of oropharynx floor. (B) Ventral view of oropharynx roof. a, rhamphotheca; b,tongue cushion; c, tongue base; d, opening of the trachea; e, larynx; f,esophagus; g, rhamphotheca; h, bony palate; i, soft palate; k, choana; m,vomer.

Fig. 6.

Measurements selected to describe the feeding behaviour of the greater rhea Rhea americana and used in the principal component analysis. Numbers in the graphs refer to parameters listed in Table 3. Measurements from the groups `gape', `lower bill', `prokinesis' and `rhynchokinesis' are correlated to the maximum gape during food pecking (gape 1; no. 1) and the maximum gape in the transport phase (gape 2; no. 2). Graphs of prokinesis, rhynchokinesis and lower bill depression are not given but they strongly resemble the pattern shown in the gape graph.

Fig. 6.

Measurements selected to describe the feeding behaviour of the greater rhea Rhea americana and used in the principal component analysis. Numbers in the graphs refer to parameters listed in Table 3. Measurements from the groups `gape', `lower bill', `prokinesis' and `rhynchokinesis' are correlated to the maximum gape during food pecking (gape 1; no. 1) and the maximum gape in the transport phase (gape 2; no. 2). Graphs of prokinesis, rhynchokinesis and lower bill depression are not given but they strongly resemble the pattern shown in the gape graph.

The third principal component mainly describes the handling of the food item, which affects the amount of depression of the lower bill (lower bill at Gape 1), position of the food item between the bills during the upward movement of the head (food level, food level s.d.), and amplitude of cranial kinesis (e.g. prokinesis at Gape 1 and 2, rhynchokinesis at Gape 2; Table 6). Differences between food types on PC3 are only found between large apples and seeds (food type 1 vs 4, t-test, Bonferroni correction, P<0.05). However, no clear trends can be determined with a change in size of the food types.

Cranial kinesis

To test the presence of kinesis in the skull of the rhea, several measurements were taken. The movement between the cranium and the upper bill around the point where the nasal–frontal hinge would be in prokinetic birds was measured, and will be further referred to as prokinetic movement(Fig. 1A). A second measure of kinesis was the movement between the rostral and caudal part of the upper bill with the border of the two parts in the bending region of the upper bill. Movement of the rostral part relative to the caudal part of the upper bill will be referred to as the rhynchokinetic movement(Fig. 1B). Since food types are different in size, the kinesis of the upper bill was determined for each food type separately. The large apple was not used for this analysis due to the small number of repeated measurements.

Fig. 7.

Plot of PC1 against PC2 of the analysis, including drinking behaviour. Different food types are represented by different markers, and groups of a single food type are outlined. Circles, large apple; closed squares, small apple; open squares, pellets; triangles, seeds; open circles, drinking behaviour.

Fig. 7.

Plot of PC1 against PC2 of the analysis, including drinking behaviour. Different food types are represented by different markers, and groups of a single food type are outlined. Circles, large apple; closed squares, small apple; open squares, pellets; triangles, seeds; open circles, drinking behaviour.

Fig. 8.

Plot of PC1 against PC2 of the analysis of food types only. Different food types are represented by different markers, and groups of a single food type are outlined. Circles, large apple; closed squares, small apple; open squares,pellets; triangles, seeds.

Fig. 8.

Plot of PC1 against PC2 of the analysis of food types only. Different food types are represented by different markers, and groups of a single food type are outlined. Circles, large apple; closed squares, small apple; open squares,pellets; triangles, seeds.

It is assumed that maximal kinesis is observed during the large amplitude gapes when the food item is picked up or swallowed. Velocities of the head are very large during the second phase of the `Catch and Throw' movement, which makes it very difficult to determine points accurately. Because of this low accuracy and the relatively small movements in the upper bill, cranial kinesis could only be analysed accurately in the grasping phase. From repeated experiments the average pick-up cycle was calculated and plotted with the standard error. The plot of gape vs time shows a clear pattern(Fig. 9A) similar to a single food uptake cycle, and differences between the time segments are significant(ANOVA, small apple: d.f.=85, P<0.001; pellets: d.f.=90, P<0.001; seeds: d.f.=168, P<0.001; water: P<0.001).

A similar analysis was made for the lower bill movement, expressed as the depression angle (Fig. 9D). For all food types the same pattern was found, and only for the largest food type analysed (small apple) were the differences between successive time segments not significant, due to large variation (ANOVA, small apple: d.f.=86, P=NS; pellets: d.f.=89, P<0.05; seeds: d.f.=168, P<0.05; water: d.f.=100, P<0.05).

Angular measurements were also used to test the response of prokinetic and rhynchokinetic movement during the feeding cycle. Prokinetic movement showed a pattern similar to the lower bill movement but with a much smaller amplitude(Fig. 9B). However, the prokinetic movement pattern is not significant for any food type or drinking(ANOVA; small apple, d.f.=87, P=NS; pellets: d.f.=89, P=NS,seeds: d.f.=167, P=NS; water: d.f.=102, P=NS). The rhynchokinetic movement patterns can be clearly recognised except in the drinking behaviour (Fig. 9C),but are only significant for the two largest food types, small apple and pellets (ANOVA, small apple: d.f.=84, P<0.05; pellets: d.f.=88, P<0.05; seeds: d.f.=166, P=NS; water: d.f.=98, P=NS). In Table 7 the maximal changes in the mean angles of the different types of kinesis are given, showing an increase in cranial kinesis with an increase in food size.

Table 7.

Maximal change in mean of all feeding cycles of kinesis parameters

FoodNGape (cm)Lower bill (deg.)Prokinesis (deg.)Rhynchokinesis (deg.)
Small apple 11 1.8 (5.4) (1.1) 10.1 
Pellets 13 1.7 6.6 (3.7) 7.1 
Seeds 12 0.9 4.2 (1.1) (2.9) 
Water 3.7 24.5 (6.6) (4.3) 
FoodNGape (cm)Lower bill (deg.)Prokinesis (deg.)Rhynchokinesis (deg.)
Small apple 11 1.8 (5.4) (1.1) 10.1 
Pellets 13 1.7 6.6 (3.7) 7.1 
Seeds 12 0.9 4.2 (1.1) (2.9) 
Water 3.7 24.5 (6.6) (4.3) 

Values within parentheses indicate non-significant changes.

Fig. 9.

Kinesis in the upper bill of the greater rhea Rhea americana. Squares indicate mean values (N=13, except at 0.24 s, where N=12) for each parameter at a certain time for the peck up phase with a medium sized food type (3: pellets). Vertical lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. The gape (A) is represented as the distance between the bill tips. Prokinesis (B), rhynchokinesis (C) and lower bill depression (D) are represented by change in angles (no absolute values). Negative angles for prokinesis and rhynchokinesis indicate elevation of the upper bill. Lower bill depression is represented by positive values.

Fig. 9.

Kinesis in the upper bill of the greater rhea Rhea americana. Squares indicate mean values (N=13, except at 0.24 s, where N=12) for each parameter at a certain time for the peck up phase with a medium sized food type (3: pellets). Vertical lines indicate 95% confidence intervals. The gape (A) is represented as the distance between the bill tips. Prokinesis (B), rhynchokinesis (C) and lower bill depression (D) are represented by change in angles (no absolute values). Negative angles for prokinesis and rhynchokinesis indicate elevation of the upper bill. Lower bill depression is represented by positive values.

Kinematic analysis

Our study showed clearly that the feeding behaviour of the palaeognathous birds strongly resembles the feeding patterns of neognathous birds. Some differences are found in the approach phase, the intra-oral transport and intra-pharyngeal transport. In general the tongue plays a more important role in neognathous feeding behaviour than in palaeognathous feeding behaviour. The complete intra-oropharyngeal transport phase in palaeognathous birds is achieved by a single `Catch and Throw' movement, which is in large contrast with the complicated `Slide and Glue' mechanism and complex transport of food through the oropharynx often used by neognathous birds. However, for large food items neognathous birds may also use a single `Catch and Throw' movement,but still show complex intra-pharyngeal transport(Zweers et al., 1994). The difference between the single `Catch and Throw' movement of neognathous and palaeognathous birds is that the former use this movement to transport the food item onto the lingual base, while the latter use it to transport the food item to the area caudal to the lingual base. Tomlinson(2000) showed that in palaeognathous feeding behaviour the final transport of the food item into the oesophagus is achieved by a single retraction of the tongue and larynx. The protraction/depression of the tongue during the `Catch and Throw' movement and the retraction during swallowing are the main functions of the tongue during feeding.

Similarly, the drinking behaviour of the rhea lacks the tongue movement present in neognathous drinking. The general drinking pattern of neognathous birds (Zweers, 1992) consists of (1) a fixation phase, in which the bird orientates its head, (2) the downstroke, in which the head is lowered towards the water, (3) the immersion phase, during which the actual water intake takes place, and (4) the upstroke,in which the head is positioned in such a way that gravitational forces facilitate transport of the water from the oropharynx into the oesophagus(swallowing). All these phases are also represented in palaeognathous drinking behaviour and similar to neognathous phases. Large differences, however, are found in the immersion phase. In the rhea there is no stationary immersion phase but a scooping motion, the bill remains widely opened, and no tongue movement is observed during this phase. Intra-oral transport during immersion in Neognathes always includes pro- and retraction of the tongue. Only during the head upstroke does a single protraction of the tongue in rhea facilitate the movement of the water into the oesophagus.

Although feeding and drinking behaviours were analysed under controlled conditions, field data show that the observed feeding behaviours are present in the natural behaviour of the rhea as well. The natural feeding and food-acquisition behaviour of all Palaeognathae, except the kiwi(Apteryx sp.), can be described as browsing, which means eating a wide variety of plant material with some occasional carnivorous food. The food preferences of the greater rhea in the wild(Martella et al., 1996)suggest that no fundamentally different feeding behaviours are required, other than the ones analysed in our study. The diet consists of a wide variety of food items, but is mainly vegetarian(Mosa, 1993; Martella et al., 1996; Quin, 1996). The assumption that the feeding behaviour is characteristic for all Palaeognathae is confimed by a number of observations. The single `Catch and Throw' feeding behaviour and both the scooping and `Catch and Throw' drinking behaviour have been observed in the greater rhea in the wild. We also observed the single `Catch and Throw' feeding behaviour in wild and captive ostriches Struthio camelus L., captive emus Dromaius novaehollandiae Latham and captive cassowaries Casuarius casuarius (L.). Although there are some differences between the diets of the various palaeognathous species, these seem due to local food availability, and not to preference or performance.

In order to determine the importance of cranial kinesis in the feeding behaviour of the palaeognathous species, we determined bending both in the area of the nasal–frontal articulation and in the upper bill itself. Our study showed that during feeding behaviour kinesis is found between the rostral and caudal part of the upper bill in rhea. No, or only very limited,bending occurs in the area of the nasal–frontal articulation, the position where in many neognathous species the naso–frontal hinge is situated. The elevation amplitude of the bill tip relative to the cranium in the rhea is similar to the elevation of the upper bill found in prokinetic neognathous birds (approximately 5–10°; Kooloos and Zweers, 1989; Heidweiller and Zweers, 1990; van den Heuvel, 1992).

One hypothesis about the role of rhynchokinesis states that it reinforces the grip on food items by simultaneously depressing the upper bill tip and elevating the lower bill tip, as found in certain Charadriiformes(Zusi, 1984). No upper bill depression is observed in the rhea, which indicates that rhynchokenesis is not used in this way in Palaeognathae.

Our video recordings of beak movement suggested that there may be a difference between neognathous rhynchokinesis and paleognathous rhynchokinesis. While a clear bending point is present in Neognathae, the upper beak in rhea seems flexible over its full length. The elevation angle of the upper bill gradually declines more caudally in the upper bill. This strongly suggests that a single hinge or narrow bending zone is not present in Palaeognathae. This conforms with the description of Zusi(1984), who named this flexibility over the full-length `central' rhynchokinesis. The relation between rhynchokinesis and the detailed anatomy of the beak is explored in an accompanying paper (Gussekloo and Bout,2005).

Phylogenetic analysis of feeding behaviour

To determine whether the feeding behaviour of the Palaeognathae is derived or primitive within modern birds, a comparison can be made with the general feeding patterns found in other tetrapods. The method of feeding in tetrapods depends on the presence of a well-developed lingual apparatus. If a well-developed lingual apparatus is absent two main types of non-lingual feeding are present within the tetrapods: inertial feeding and the feeding pattern observed in snakes (de Vree and Gans, 1994). Comparison of the feeding behaviour of the rhea with the nearest living sister group of birds, the crocodilians, shows that the feeding behaviour of the rhea is more similar to reptilian inertial feeding than the general feeding pattern of neognathous birds(Zweers et al., 1994; Cleuren and de Vree, 1992). In crocodilian intra-oral transport the tongue elevates the food item until it presses against the palate. Then gape is rapidly increased and the cranium moved forward (the avian `Catch and Throw'), while the tongue is depressed to enlarge the buccal cavity and to facilitate the transport of the food item. In the rhea the final transport of a food item into the oesophagus is achieved by a retraction of the hyolingual apparatus(Tomlinson, 2000), similar to transport in crocodilians. The fact that feeding behaviour of the rhea resembles feeding behaviour of crocodilians, and lacks certain elements found in the general feeding pattern of neognathous birds, seems to suggest that inertial feeding behaviour is basal within birds. This would agree with the widely accepted hypothesis that the Palaeognathae are the oldest offshoot in the phylogeny of modern birds (Bock,1963; Meise, 1963; Parkes and Clark, 1966; Cracraft, 1974; de Boer, 1980; Prager and Wilson, 1980; Sibley and Ahlquist, 1981; McGowan, 1984; Feduccia, 1985; Handford and Mares, 1985;Elzanowski, 1986; Houde, 1986; Bledsoe, 1988; Caspers et al., 1994; Lee et al., 1997; van Tuinen et al., 1998; Simon et al., 2004). However,lingual feeding is found in the more primitive amphibians(de Vree and Gans, 1994), and present in many reptilians (Bramble and Wake, 1985; Reilly and Lauder,1990; Herrel et al.,1996). As crocodiles are a very distant sister group of birds the possibility remains that crocodiles and Paleaognathae are specialized inertial feeders and that lingual feeding is the most primitive avian feeding mechanism. Tetrapod inertial feeding is believed to have evolved many times independently within vertebrates (de Vree and Gans, 1994). The simple movement patterns of the tongue in rhea may be the consequence of a reduction in size related to efficient `Catch and Throw' feeding behaviour. Since it cannot unambiguously be determined whether the feeding pattern of the palaeognathous birds is primitive or a specific adaptation, these data cannot be used to determine the phylogenetic position of the Palaeognathae.

General discussion

From the comparison of feeding patterns we conclude that the feeding and drinking behaviours of the rhea resemble those of neognaths, but lack certain elements found in the general feeding pattern of neognathous birds, especially with respect to tongue movements. We found no elements in the feeding behaviour that might impose additional functional demands on the PPC, nor are any of the behavioural elements investigated more demanding than in neognathous feeding. This indicates that the specific morphology of the PPC is not the result of specific functional demands from palaeognathous feeding behaviour. Also the hypothesised role of rhynchokinesis in relation to the cranial morphology could not be confirmed. Central rhynchokinesis is present in the upper bill, but does not play an important role in improving grip on the food item or in increasing the gape. It must therefore be concluded that the kinetic feature of the bill is not the factor that determined the morphology of the PPC.

Alternative explanations for the presence of the characteristic PPC complex have been suggested. Bock(1963) proposed that the special morphology of the PPC might be an adaptation to the high impact forces on the bill during pecking. Our movement analysis showed that the rhea is capable of controlling the impact force of pecking. The head hits the ground at approximately 11.30 m s–2. The mean calculated impact force was 2.83 N and the maximum did not exceed 7.54 N. Using a compressive strength of 170×106 Pa for bone we can calculate that a cross-sectional area of the bones of just 0.05 mm2 is sufficient to withstand these forces. It seems clear that this area is many times smaller than the actual cross-sectional area in the skull of the rhea.

Another explanation is that the morphology of the palaeognathous PPC is the result of selection forces that are not directly related to feeding, but do affect the morphology of the PPC indirectly. While Paleognathae are often believed to be basal to Neognathae, an alternative hypothesis states that Palaeognathae have actually a derived phylogenetic position and have evolved through neoteny from a flying ancestor (de Beer, 1956). The hypothesis on the neotenous origin of the Palaeognathae was recently revived by physiological/ontogenetic data(Dawson et al., 1994) and molecular systematics (Mindell et al.,1997; Härlid and Arnason,1999). The physiological/ontogenetic experiments showed that induced neoteny in neognathous birds results in a morphology of the PPC that was similar to that of the Palaeognathae, while the molecular systematic data show a derived position of the Palaeognathae within the Neognathae and not a basal position of the group. A comparison of the cranial morphology of the Palaeognathae with different developmental stages of neognatous birds showed,however, that not a single developmental stage resembles the palaeognathous configuration (Gussekloo and Bout,2002). This is a clear indication that the palaeognathous PPC is not the result of neoteny.

A third hypothesis on the origin of the special morphology of the palaeognathous PPC proposes that the morphology of the extant palaeognathous PPC is the result of the continuous reduction of bony and ligamentous elements in the lateral aspect of the skull(Gussekloo and Zweers, 1999). Although birds in general have less bony and ligamentous elements in the lateral aspect of the skull than closely related groups such as dinosaurs and other reptiles, Palaeognathae have even less than most birds. Compared to Neognathae, Palaeognathae lack a clear Ligamentum postorbitale and the lateral bar of the upper bill (Bock,1964; Zusi, 1984). The reduction of these elements might have resulted in a relatively unstable configuration of the upper bill, especially under conditions of external loading. The only type of food acquisition that is not covered by our study is pulling leaves off plants. It is possible that this way of feeding imposes special functional demands on the construction of the upper beak. The removal of leaves is mainly achieved by neck motion, and generates external forces on the upper bill. While preliminary observations showed that the transport of the food items used in the present study is very similar to the transport of grass or leaves that are removed from the plant, it is possible that the morphology of the upper bill of the Palaeognathae is adapted to oppose the reaction forces during pulling (see also Gussekloo and Bout, 2005). Although it seems that such a force regime is not unique for Paleognathae(e.g. grazing geese, pulling off berries by passerines), it is unique when combined with the absence of lateral bars in the skull. Since the Palaeognathae could not counteract the external forces by reinforcing these lateral elements, it may have been necessary to reinforce the unstable upper bill configuration in the ventral elements, resulting in a more rigid PPC. As a consequence of this reinforcement of the ventral plane of the upper bill,active kinesis of the upper bill may also become limited. Additional experiments to test this hypothesis are described in the accompanying paper(Gussekloo and Bout,2005).

Beddard, F. (
1898
).
The Structure and Classification of Birds
. London: Longmans, Green and Company.
Bledsoe, A. H. (
1988
). A phylogenetic analysis of postcranial skeletal characters of the ratite birds.
Ann. Carnegie Mus.
57
,
73
-90.
Bock, W. J. (
1963
). The cranial evidence for Ratite affinities.
Proceedings of the XIIIth International Ornithological Congress
(ed. C. G. Sibley, J. J. Hickey and M. B. Hickey), pp.
39
-54. Baton Rouge, FL: American Ornithologists' Union.
Bock, W. J. (
1964
). Kinetics of the avian skull.
J. Morphol.
114
,
1
-42.
Bramble, D. and Wake, D. B. (
1985
). Feeding mechanisms of lower tetrapods. In
Functional Vertebrate Morphology
(ed. M. Hildebrand, D. Bramble, K. Liem, and D. B. Wake), pp.
230
-261. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruning, D. F. (
1974
). Social structure and reproductive behaviour in the greater rhea.
Living Bird
13
,
251
-294.
Caspers, G.-J., Wattel, J. and de Jong, W. W.(
1994
). Alpha A-crystallin sequences group tinamou with ratites.
Mol. Biol. Evol.
11
,
711
-713.
Cleuren, J. and de Vree, F. (
1992
). Kinematics of the jaw and hyolingual apparatus during feeding in Caiman crocodilus.
J. Morphol.
212
,
141
-154.
Cracraft, J. (
1974
). Phylogeny and evolution of the ratite birds.
Ibis
116
,
494
-521.
Dawson, A., McNaughton, F. J., Goldsmith, A. R. and Degen, A. A. (
1994
). Ratite-like neoteny induced by neonatal thyroidectomy of European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris.
J. Zool.
232
,
633
-639.
de Beer, G. (
1956
). The evolution of ratites.
Bull. Br. Mus. Nat. Hist. Zool.
4
,
5
-70.
de Boer, L. E. M. (
1980
). Do the chromosomes of the kiwi provide evidence for a monophyletic origin of ratites?
Nature
287
,
84
-85.
Elzanowski, A. (
1989
). Ontogeny and evolution of the ratites. In
Acta XIX Congresseus Internationalis Ornithologici,
Vol.
2
(ed. H. Ouellet),pp.
2037
-2046. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.
Feduccia, A. (
1985
). The morphological evidence for ratite monophyly, fact or fiction. In
Proceedings of the 18th International Ornithological Congress
(ed. V. D. Ilyichev and V. M. Gavrilov), pp.
184
-190. Moscow: Academy of Sciences of the USSR.
Furbringer, M. (
1888
).
Untersuchungen zur Morphologie und Systematik der Vogel
. Amsterdam: van Holkema.
Gadow, H. (
1892
). On the classification of birds.
Proc. Zool. Soc. London
1892
,
229
-256.
Gussekloo, S. W. S. and Bout, R. G. (
2002
). Non-neotenous origin of the palaeognathous (Aves) pterygoid-palate complex.
Can. J. Zool.
80
,
1491
-1497.
Gussekloo, S. W. S. and Bout, R. G. (
2005
). Cranial kinesis in palaeognathous birds.
J. Exp. Biol.
208
,
3409
-3419.
Gussekloo, S. W. S. and Zweers, G. A. (
1999
). The paleognathous pterygoid-palatinum complex. A true character?
Neth. J. Zool.
49
,
29
-43.
Gussekloo, S. W. S., Vosselman, M. G. and Bout, R. G.(
2001
). Three-dimensional kinematics of skeletal elements in avian prokinetic and rhynchokinetic skulls determined by roentgen stereophotogrammetry.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1735
-1744.
Handford, P. and Mares, M. A. (
1985
). The mating systems of ratites and tinamous: an evolutionary perspective.
Biol. J. Linn. Soc.
25
,
77
-104.
Härlid, A. and Arnason, U. (
1999
). Analyses of mitochondrial DNA nest ratite birds within the Neognathae:supporting a neotenous origin of ratite morphological characters.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
266
,
305
-309.
Heidweiller, J. and Zweers, G. A. (
1990
). Drinking mechanisms in the zebra finch and the bengalese finch.
Condor
92
,
1
-28.
Herrel, A., Cleuren, J. and de Vree, F. (
1996
). Kinematics of feeding in the lizard Agama stellio.
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
1727
-1742.
Hofer, H. (
1954
). Neue Untersuchungen zur Kopf Morphologie. In
Acta XI Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici
(ed. A. Portmann and E. Sutter), pp.
104
-137. Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag.
Houde, P. (
1986
). Ostrich ancestors found in the nothern hemisphere suggest new hypothesis of ratite origins.
Nature
324
,
563
-565.
Huxley, T. H. (
1867
). On the classification of birds; and on the taxonomic value of the modifications of certain of the cranial bones observable in that class.
Proc. Zool. Soc.
27
,
415
-472.
Kooloos, J. G. M. and Zweers, G. A. (
1989
). Mechanics of drinking in the mallard (Anas platyrhynchos, Anatidae).
J. Morphol.
199
,
327
-347.
Lee, K., Feinstein, J. and Cracraft, J. (
1997
). The phylogeny of ratite birds: resolving conflicts between molecular and morphological data sets. In
Avian Molecular Evolution and Systematics
(ed. D. P. Mindell), pp.
173
-195. San Diego: Academic Press.
Martella, M. B., Renison, D. and Navarro, J. L.(
1995
). Vigilance in the greater rhea: effects of vegetation height and group size.
J. Field Ornithol.
66
,
215
-220.
Martella, M. B., Navarro, J. L., Gonnet, J. M. and Monge, S. A. (
1996
). Diet of greater rheas in an agrecosystem of central Argentina.
J. Wildl. Manage.
60
,
586
-592.
McDowell, S. (
1948
). The bony palate of birds. Part I, the Paleognathae.
Auk
65
,
520
-549.
McGowan, C. (
1984
). Evolutionary relationships of ratites and carinates: evidence from ontogeny of the tarsus.
Nature
307
,
733
-735.
Meise, W. (
1963
). Verhalten der Straussartigen und Monophylie der Ratitae. In
Proceedings of the XIIIth International Ornithological Congress
(ed. C. G. Sibley, J. J. Hickey and M. B. Hickey), pp.
115
-125. Baton Rouge,FL: American Ornithologists' Union.
Merrem, B. (
1813
). Systematis naturalis Avium.
Abhandlungen der Köninglichen Akademie der Wissenschaften zu Berlin
,
237
-259.
Mindell, D. P., Sorenson, M. D., Huddleston, C. J., Miranda, H. C., Knight, A., Sawchuk, S. J. and Yuri, T. (
1997
). Phylogenetic relationships among and within select avian orders based on mitochondrial DNA. In
Avian Molecular Evolution and Systematics
(ed. D. P. Mindell), pp.
213
-247. San Diego: Academic Press.
Mosa, S. G. (
1993
). Fall and winter diet and habitat preferences of the Andean Tinamou (Nothura pentlandii) in the Northwest Argentina.
Stud. Neotrop. Environ.
28
,
123
-128.
Parkes, K. C. and Clark, G. A. (
1966
). An additional character linking ratites and tinamous, and an interpretation of their monophyly.
Condor
68
,
459
-471.
Prager, E. M. and Wilson, A. C. (
1980
). Phylogenetic relationships and rates of evolution in birds. In
Acta 17th International Ornithological Congress
(ed. R. N. Shring),pp.
1209
-1214. Berlin: Deutsche Ornithologen-Gesellschaft.
Quin, B. R. (
1996
). Diet and habitat of emus Dromaius novaehollandiae in the Grampians ranges, South-western Victoria.
Emu
96
,
114
-122.
Raikow, R. J. (
1968
). Maintenance behaviour of the common rhea.
Wilson Bull.
80
,
312
-319.
Raikow, R. J. (
1969
). Sexual and agonistic behaviour of the common rhea.
Wilson Bull.
81
,
196
-206.
Reboreda, J. C. and Fernandez, G. J. (
1997
). Sexual, seasonal and group size differences in the allocation of time between vigilance and feeding in the Greater Rhea, Rhea americana.
Ethology
103
,
198
-207.
Reilly, S. M. and Lauder, G. V. (
1990
). The evolution of tetrapod prey transport behavior: kinematic homologies in feeding function.
Evolution
44
,
1542
-1557.
Sibley, C. G. and Ahlquist, J. E. (
1981
). The phylogeny and relationships of the ratite birds as indicated by DNA-DNA hybridization. In
Evolution Today, Proceedings of the Second International Congress of Systematic and Evolutionary Biology
(ed. G. G. E. Scudder and J. L. Reveal), pp.
301
-335. Pittsburgh: Hunt Institute for Botanical Documentation.
Simon, J., Laurent, S., Grolleau, P., Thorval, P., Soubieux, D. and Rasschaert, D. (
2004
). Evolution of preproinsulin gene in birds.
Mol. Phyogenet. Evol.
30
,
755
-766.
Simonetta, A. M. (
1960
). On the mechanical implications of the avian skull and their bearing on the evolution and classification of birds.
Q. Rev. Biol.
35
,
206
-220.
Tomlinson, C. A. (
2000
). Feeding in palaeognathous birds. In
Feeding: Form, Function, and Evolution in Tetrapod Vertebrates
(ed. K. Schwenk), pp.
359
-394. San Diego: Academic Press.
Tuinen, M. van Sibley, C. G. and Hedges, S. B.(
1998
). Phylogeny and biography of ratite birds inferred from DNA sequences of the mitochondrial ribosomal genes.
Mol. Biol. Evol.
17
,
451
-457.
van den Heuvel, W. F. (
1992
). Kinetics of the skull in the chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus).
Neth. J. Zool.
42
,
561
-582.
Vree, F. de and Gans, C. (
1994
). Feeding in tetrapods. In
Advances in Comparative and Environmental Physiology 18: Biomechanics of Feeding in Vertebrates
(ed. V. L. Bels, M. Chardon and P. Vandewalle), pp.
93
-113. Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Zusi, R. L. (
1984
). A functional and evolutionary analysis of rhynchokinesis in birds.
Smithson. Contrib. Zool.
395
,
1
-40.
Zweers, G. A. (
1992
). Behavioural mechanisms of avian drinking.
Neth. J. Zool.
42
,
60
-84.
Zweers, G. A., Berkhoudt, H. and Vanden Berge, J. C.(
1994
). Behavioral mechanisms of avian feeding.
Adv. Comp. Physiol. Biol.
18
,
243
-279.